creators_name: Gabora, Liane creators_name: Aerts, Diederik creators_id: liane.gabora@ubc.ca creators_id: diraerts@vub.ac.be type: journalp datestamp: 2010-01-30 03:42:33 lastmod: 2011-03-11 08:57:35 metadata_visibility: show title: A model of the emergence and evolution of integrated worldviews ispublished: pub subjects: cog-psy subjects: cogarch subjects: evol-psy full_text_status: public keywords: cognitive development, concepts, context, cultural evolution, entanglement, evolutionary psychology, conceptual integraton, quantum interaction, SCOP formalism, worldview abstract: It is proposed that the ability of humans to flourish in diverse environments and evolve complex cultures reflects the following two underlying cognitive transitions. The transition from the coarse-grained associative memory of Homo habilis to the fine-grained memory of Homo erectus enabled limited representational redescription of perceptually similar episodes, abstraction, and analytic thought, the last of which is modeled as the formation of states and of lattices of properties and contexts for concepts. The transition to the modern mind of Homo sapiens is proposed to have resulted from onset of the capacity to spontaneously and temporarily shift to an associative mode of thought conducive to interaction amongst seemingly disparate concepts, modeled as the forging of conjunctions resulting in states of entanglement. The fruits of associative thought became ingredients for analytic thought, and vice versa. The ratio of associative pathways to concepts surpassed a percolation threshold resulting in the emergence of a self-modifying, integrated internal model of the world, or worldview. date: 2009 date_type: published publication: Journal of Mathematical Psychology volume: 53 number: 5 refereed: TRUE referencetext: \item Aerts, D. (1982). Description of many physical entities Without the paradoxes encountered in quantum mechanics. \textit{Foundations of Physics, 12,} 1131-1170. \item Aerts, D. (1983). Classical theories and non classical theories as a special case of a more general theory, \textit{Journal of Mathematical Physics, 24,} 2441-2453. \item Aerts, D. (2007). General quantum modeling of combining concepts: A quantum field model in Fock space. Archive address and link: http://arxiv.org/abs/0705.1740. \item Aerts, D. (2009). Quantum structure in cognition. Journal of Mathematical Psychology (submitted). Aerts, D., Broekaert, J. and Smets, S. (1999a). The liar paradox in a quantum mechanical perspective. {\it Foundations of Science, 4}, pp. 115-132. Aerts, D., Broekaert, J., Smets, S. (1999b). A quantum structure description of the liar paradox. {\it International Journal of Theoretical Physics, 38}, pp. 3231-3239. \item Aerts, D. \& Gabora, L. (2005a). A theory of concepts and their combinations I: The structure of the sets of contexts and properties. \textit{Kybernetes, 34}(1\&2), 167-191. \item Aerts, D. \& Gabora, L. (2005b). A theory of concepts and their combinations II: A Hilbert space representation. \textit{Kybernetes, 34}(1\&2), 192-221. \item Aiello, L. \& Dunbar, R. (1993). Neocortex size, group size, and the evolution of language. \textit{Current Anthropology, 34}, 184-193. \item Ashby, F. G., \& Ell, S. W. (2002). Single versus multiple systems of learning and memory. In J. Wixted \& H. Pashler (Eds.) \textit{Stevens? handbook of experimental psychology: Volume 4 Methodology in experimental psychology}. New York: Wiley. \item Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., \& Tooby, J. (1992). \textit{The adapted mind.} New York: Oxford University Press. \item Bar-Yosef, O. (1994). The contribution of southwest Asia to the study of the origin of modern humans. In M. Nitecki \& D. Nitecki (Eds.) \textit{Origins of anatomically modern humans}, Plenum Press. Bouissac, P. (1998). Converging parallels: Semiotics and psychology in evolutionary perspective. \textit{Theory and Psychology}, 8(6), 731-753. \item Beltrametti, E. \& Cassinelli, G. (1981). \textit{The logic of quantum mechanics.} Boston: Addison-Wesley. \item Bickerton, D. (1990). \textit{Language and species.} Chicago: University of Chicago Press. \item Barsalou, L.W. (1982). Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts. \textit{Memory \& Cognition, 10}, 82-93. \item Bruza, P. \& Cole, R. (2005). Quantum logic of semantic space: An exploratory investigation of context effects in practical reasoning. In S. Artemov, H. Barringer, A. S. d'Avila Garcez, L. C. Lamb and J. Woods (Eds.) \textit{We Will Show Them: Essays in Honour of Dov Gabbay}. College Publications, 1, pp. 339?361. Bruza, P. Kitto, K. Nelson, D. \& McEvoy, C. (in press). Is there something quantum-like about the human mental lexicon? Journal of Mathematical Psychology. \item Busemeyer, J. R., Wang, Z., \& Townsend, J. T. (2006). Quantum dynamics of human decision making. \textit{Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 50}, 220-242. \item Busemeyer, J. R. , Matthew, M., \& Wang, Z. (2006) A Quantum Information Processing Theory Explanation of Disjunction Effects. {\it Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society}. \item Busemeyer, J. R. \& Wang, Z. (2007). Quantum Information Processing Explanation for Interactions between Inferences and Decisions. \textit{Proceedings of the AAAI Spring Symposium on Quantum Interaction} (pp. 91-97). Stanford University, March 26-28. \item Carey, S. (1985). \textit{Conceptual change in childhood}. Cambridge: MIT Press. \item Churchland, P. S. \& Sejnowski, T. (1992). \textit{The computational brain.} Cambridge MA: MIT Press. \item Corballis, M. C. (1991). \textit{The lopsided ape: Evolution of the generative mind.} Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. \item Davis, J., \& Rovee-Collier, C. (1983). Alleviated forgetting of a learned contingency in 8-week-old infants. \textit{Developmental Psychology, 19}, 353-365. \item Dawkins, R. (1976). \textit{The selfish gene}. Oxford: Oxford University Press. \item Deacon, T. (1997). \textit{The symbolic species}. New York: Norton. [p. 751] \item Dennett, D. (1978). \textit{Brainstorms}. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. \item Dikranjan, D., Giuli, E. \& Tozzi, A. (1988). Topological categories and closure operators. \textit{Questiones Mathematics, 11}(3), 323-337. \item Donald, M. (1991) \textit{Origins of the modern mind.} Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press. \item Edelman, G. E. (1993). \textit{Bright Air, Brilliant Fire: On the Matter of Mind.} New York: Basic Books. \item Erdos, P. \& Renyi, A. (1959). \textit{On the random graphs}. 1(6), Institute of Mathematics, University of Debrecenians, Debrecar, Hungary. \item Erdos, P. \& Renyi, A. (1960). \textit{On the evolution of random graphs}. Institute of Mathematics, Hungarian Academy of Sciences Publication number 5. \item Fauconnier, G. \& Turner, M. (2002). \textit{The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind's hidden complexities.} New York: Basic Books. \item Gabora, L. (1998). A tentative scenario for the origin of culture. \textit{Psycoloquy, 9}(67). \item Gabora, L. (1999). Weaving, bending, patching, mending the fabric of reality: A cognitive science perspective on worldview inconsistency. \textit{Foundations of Science, 3}(2), 395-428. \item Gabora, L. (2000). Conceptual closure: Weaving memories into an interconnected worldview. In G. Van de Vijver and J. Chandler (Eds.) \textit{Closure: Emergent Organizations and their Dynamics. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 901}, 42-53. \item Gabora, L. (2002a). The beer can theory of creativity. In P. Bentley \& D. Corne (Eds.) \textit{Creative Evolutionary Systems,} (pp. 147-161). San Francisco CA: Morgan Kauffman. \item Gabora, L. (2002b). Cognitive mechanisms underlying the creative process. In T. Hewett and T. Kavanagh (Eds.) \textit{Proceedings of the Fourth International Conference on Creativity and Cognition,} (pp. 126-133). October 13-16, Loughborough University, UK. \item Gabora, L. (2003). Contextual focus: A cognitive explanation for the cultural transition of the Middle/Upper Paleolithic. In (R. Alterman \& D. Hirsch, Eds.) \textit{Proceedings of the 25th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society}, Boston MA, 31 July-2 August. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. \item Gabora, L. (2004). Ideas are not replicators but interrelated networks of them are. \textit{Biology and Philosophy, 19}(1), 127-143. \item Gabora, L. (2005). Creative thought as a non-Darwinian evolutionary process. \textit{Journal of Creative Behavior, 39}(4), 65-87. \item Gabora, L. (2006). Self-other organization: Why early life did not evolve through natural selection. \textit{Journal of Theoretical Biology, 241} (3), 443-450. \item Gabora, L. (2008). The cultural evolution of socially situated cognition. \textit{Cognitive Systems Research, 9}(1-2), 104-113. \item Gabora, L. \& Aerts, D. (2002). Contextualizing concepts using a mathematical generalization of the quantum formalism. \textit{Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial Intelligence, 14}(4), 327-358. \item Gabora, L. \& Aerts, D. (2005). Evolution as context-driven actualization of potential: Toward an interdisciplinary theory of change of state. \textit{Interdisciplinary Science Reviews, 30}(1), 69-88. \item Gabora, L., Rosch, E., \& Aerts, D. (2008). Toward an ecological theory of concepts. \textit{Ecological Psychology, 20}(1), 84-116. \item G\"ardenfors, P. (2000). \textit{Conceptual Spaces: The geometry of thought.} Cambridge, MA: MIT press. \item Gelman, R. (1993). A rational-constructivist account of early learning about numbers and objects. In D. Medin (Ed.) \textit{Learning and motivation,} (pp. 61-96). New York: Academic Press. \item Hampton, S. J. (2004). Domina mismatches, scruffy engineering, exaptations and spandrels. \textit{Theory and Psychology 14}(2), 147-166. \item Hancock, P. J. B., Smith, L. S. \& Phillips, W. A. (1991). A biologically supported error-correcting learning rule. {\it Neural Computation, 3}(2), 201-212. \item Holden, S.B. \& Niranjan, M. (1997). Average-case learning curves for radial basis function networks. {\it Neural Computation, 9}(2), 441-460. \item Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). {\it Mental models.} Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press. \item Kanerva, P. (1988). \textit{Sparse, distributed memory.} Cambridge MA: MIT Press. \item Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1990). Constraints on representational change: Evidence from children?s drawing. \textit{Cognition, 34}, 57-83. \item Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). \textit{Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science}. Cambridge MA: MIT Press. \item Kauffman, S. (1993). \textit{Origins of order}. Oxford: Oxford University Press. \item Keil, F. C. (1995). The growth of causal understandings of natural kinds. In S. Sperber, D. Premack, \& A. Premack (Eds.) \textit{Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate,} (pp. 234-262). Cambridge: Oxford University Press. \item Klein, R. (1989). Biological and behavioral perspectives on modern human origins in South Africa. In P. Mellars \& C. Stringe (Eds.) \textit{The human revolution}. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. \item Langton, C. G. (1992) Life at the edge of chaos. In (C. G. Langton, C. Taylor, J. D. Farmer \& S. Rasmussen, eds.) \textit{Artificial life II}. Boston: Addison-Wesley. \item Leakey, R. (1984). \textit{The origins of humankind}. New York: Science Masters Basic Books. \item Leslie, A. (2000). \textit{Theory of mind as a mechanism of selective attention}. Rutgers University Tech Report 061. \item Lieberman, P. (1991). \textit{Uniquely human: The evolution of speech, thought, and selfless behavior.} Boston: Harvard University Press. \item Lu, Y.W., Sundararajan, N. \& Saratchandran, P. (1997). A sequential learning scheme for function approximation using minimal radial basis function neural networks. {\it Neural Computation, 9}(2), 461-478. \item Maturana, H. \& Varela, F. (1973/1980). \textit{Autopoeisis and cognition: The realization of the living}. Boston: Reidel. \item Matzel, L., Collin, C., \& Alkon, D. (1992). Biophysical and behavioral correlates of memory storage: Degradation and reactivation. \textit{Behavioral Neuroscience, 106}, 954-963. \item McBrearty, S. \& Brooks, A. S. (2000). The revolution that wasn?t: A new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. \textit{Journal of Human Evolution 39}(5), 453-563. \item Mednick, S. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. \textit{Psychological Review, 69,} 220?232. \item Mellars, P. (1973). The character of the middle-upper transition in south-west France. In C. Renfrew (Ed.) \textit{The explanation of culture change}. London: Duckworth. \item Mellars, P. (1989a). Technological changes in the Middle-Upper Paleolithic transition: Economic, social, and cognitive perspectives. In P. Mellars \& C. Stringer (Eds.) \textit{The human revolution}. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. \item Mellars, P. (1989b). Major issues in the emergence of modern humans. \textit{Current Anthropology, 30}, 349-385. \item Mikkulainen (1997). Neural network perspectives on cognition and adaptive robotics. In (A Brown, Ed.) \textit{Natural language processing with subsymbolic neural networks.} Bristol UK, Philadelphia: Institute of Physics Press. \item Mithen, S. (1996). \textit{The prehistory of the mind: A search for the origins of art, science, and religion.} London: Thames \& Hudson. \item Mithen, S. (1998). A creative explosion? Theory of mind, language, and the disembodied mind of the Upper Paleolithic. In S. Mithen (Ed.) \textit{Creativity in human evolution and prehistory}. New York: Routledge. \item Morowitz, H. J. (2002). \textit{The Emergence of Everything: How the World Became Complex.} Oxford University Press, New York. \item Neisser, U. (1963). The multiplicity of thought. \textit{British Journal of Psychology, 54}, 1-14. \item Nelson, D. L. \& McEvoy, C. L. (2007). Activation at a distance is not spooky anymore: The conjunction of entangled associative structures and context. \textit{Proceedings of the AAAI Spring Symposium on Quantum Interaction,} Stanford University, March 26-28. \item Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L. \& Pointer, L. (2003). Spreading activation or spooky action at a distance. \textit{Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 29,} 42?51. \item Osherson , D. N. \& Smith, E. E. (1981). On the adequacy of prototype theory as a theory of concepts. \textit{Cognition, 9}, 35-58. \item Piaget 1926). \textit{The language and thought of the child}. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. \item Piron, C. (1976). \textit{Foundations of Quantum Physics.} Reading, MA: W.A. Benjamin. \item Rosch (1978). Principles of categorization. In Rosch, E. \& Lloyd, B. (Eds.) \textit{Cognition and categorization}. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. \item Rovee-Collier, C., Hartshorn, K., \& DiRubbo, M. (1999). Long-term maintenance of infant memory. \textit{Developmental Psychobiology, 35}, 91-102. \item Ruff, C., Trinkaus, E. \& Holliday, T. (1997). Body mass and encephalization in Pleistocene Homo. \textit{Nature, 387,} 173-176. \item Sloman, S. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of Reasoning. \textit{Psychological Bulletin, 9}(1), 3-22. \item Smolensky, P. (1988). On the proper treatment of connectionism. \textit{Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11,} 1?43. \item Soffer, O. (1994). Ancestral lifeways in Eurasia---The Middle and Upper Paleolithic records. In M. Nitecki \& D. Nitecki, (Eds.) \textit{Origins of anatomically modern humans.} New York: Plenum Press. \item Spelke, E. S. (2000). Core knowledge. \textit{American Psychologist, 55}(11), 1233-1243. \item Spoor, F. Leakey, M. G., Gathogo, P. N., Brown, F. H., Anton, S. C., McDougall, I., Kiarie, C., Manthi, F. K. and Leakey, L. N. (2007). Implications of new early homo fossils from ileret, east of lake turkana, kenya. \textit{Nature, 448,} 688-691. \item Stringer, C. \& Gamble, C. (1993). \textit{In search of the Neanderthals.} London: Thames \& Hudson. \item Thompson, E.(2007). \textit{Mind in Life - Biology, Phenomenology and the Sciences of Mind.} Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press. \item Vetsigian, K., Woese, C., \& Goldenfeld, N. (2006). Collective evolution and the genetic code. \textit{Proceedings of the New York Academy of Science USA, 103,} 10696?10701. \item W?chtersh?user, G. (1992). Groundwork for an evolutionary biochemistry: the iron-sulfur world. \textit{Prog. Biophys. Molec. Biol. 58,} 85-201. \item Weber, B. H. (1998). Emergence of life and biological selection from the perspective of complex systems dynamics. In: G. van de Vijver, S. N. Salthe \& M. Delpos (Eds.) \textit{Evolutionary Systems: Biological and Epistemological Perspectives on Selection and Self-Organization.} Kluwer, Dordrecht. \item Weber, B. H. (2000). Closure in the emergence and evolution of life: Multiple discourses or one? In: J. L. R. Chandler \& G. Van de Vijver (Eds.) \textit{Closure: Emergent Organizations and their Dynamics, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 901,} pp. 132-138. \item Weber, B.H., \& Depew, J.D. (1996). Natural selection and self-organization. \textit{Biology and Philosophy, 11}(1), 33-65. \item White, R. (1982). Rethinking the Middle/Upper Paleolithic transition. \textit{Current Anthropology, 23}, 169-189. \item White, R. (1993). Technological and social dimensions of 'Aurignacian-age' body ornaments across Europe. In H. Knecht, A. Pike-Tay, \& R. White (Eds.) \textit{Before Lascaux: The complex record of the Early Upper Paleolithc.} New York: CRC Press. \item Widdows, D. S. (2003). Orthogonal negation in vector spaces for modeling word meanings and document retrieval. In \textit{Proceedings of the 41st Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics} (p. 136-143), Sapporo Japan, July 7-12. \item Widdows, D. \& Peters, S. (2003). Word vectors and quantum logic: experiments with negation and disjunction. In \textit{Eighth Meeting of the Association for the Mathematics of Language} (p. 141-154), Bloomington IN: Association for Computational Linguistics. \item Williams, R. J. P. \& Frausto da Silva, J. J. R. (1999). \textit{Bringing Chemistry to Life: From Matter to Man.} Oxford University Press, Oxford. \item Williams, R. J. P. \& Frausto da Silva, J. J. R. (2002). The systems approach to evolution. \textit{Biochem. Biophys. Res. Comm. 297,} 689-699. \item Williams, R. J. P. \&Frausto da Silva, J. J. R. (2003). Evolution was chemically constrained. \textit{J. Theor. Biol. 220,} 323-343. \item Willshaw, D. J. \& Dayan, P. (1990). Optimal plasticity from matrix memory: What goes up must come down. {\it Journal of Neural Computation, 2}, 85-93. \item Woese, C.R. (2002). On the evolution of cells. \textit{Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99}(13), 8742-8747. citation: Gabora, Dr. Liane and Aerts, Dr. Diederik (2009) A model of the emergence and evolution of integrated worldviews. [Journal (Paginated)] document_url: http://cogprints.org/6768/1/gabora_aerts_jmp09.pdf