Cogprints

The Content and Acquisition of Lexical Concepts

Horsey, Richard (2006) The Content and Acquisition of Lexical Concepts. [Thesis]

Full text available as:

[img] PDF
1298Kb

Abstract

This thesis aims to develop a psychologically plausible account of concepts by integrating key insights from philosophy (on the metaphysical basis for concept possession) and psychology (on the mechanisms underlying concept acquisition). I adopt an approach known as informational atomism, developed by Jerry Fodor. Informational atomism is the conjunction of two theses: (i) informational semantics, according to which conceptual content is constituted exhaustively by nomological mind–world relations; and (ii) conceptual atomism, according to which (lexical) concepts have no internal structure. I argue that informational semantics needs to be supplemented by allowing content-constitutive rules of inference (“meaning postulates”). This is because the content of one important class of concepts, the logical terms, is not plausibly informational. And since, it is argued, no principled distinction can be drawn between logical concepts and the rest, the problem that this raises is a general one. An immediate difficulty is that Quine’s classic arguments against the analytic/synthetic distinction suggest that there can be no principled basis for distinguishing content-constitutive rules from the rest. I show that this concern can be overcome by taking a psychological approach: there is a fact of the matter as to whether or not a particular inference is governed by a mentally-represented inference rule, albeit one that analytic philosophy does not have the resources to determine. I then consider the implications of this approach for concept acquisition. One mechanism underlying concept acquisition is the development of perceptual detectors for the objects that we encounter. I investigate how this might work, by drawing on recent ideas in ethology on ‘learning instincts’, and recent insights into the neurological basis for perceptual learning. What emerges is a view of concept acquisition as involving a complex interplay between innate constraints and environmental input. This supports Fodor’s recent move away from radical concept nativism: concept acquisition requires innate mechanisms, but does not require that concepts themselves be innate.

Item Type:Thesis
Keywords:concepts, Fodor, atomism, content, concept nativism, informational semantics, Dretske, lexical concepts, meaning postulates, analytic/synthetic, concept acquisition, learning instincts
Subjects:Psychology > Cognitive Psychology
Philosophy > Philosophy of Mind
ID Code:2229
Deposited By: Horsey, Richard
Deposited On:13 Aug 2006
Last Modified:11 Mar 2011 08:54

References in Article

Select the SEEK icon to attempt to find the referenced article. If it does not appear to be in cogprints you will be forwarded to the paracite service. Poorly formated references will probably not work.

Achinstein, P. 1974. The identity conditions of properties. American Philosophical Quarterly, 11: 257–275.

Ahissar, M. 1999. Perceptual learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8: 124–128.

Ahissar, M. & S. Hochstein 1993. Attentional control of early perceptual learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 90: 5718–5722.

Ahissar, M. & S. Hochstein 1996. Learning pop-out detection: specificities to stimulus caracteristics. Vision Research, 36: 3487–3500.

Ahissar, M. & S. Hochstein 1997. Task difficulty and the specificity of perceptual learning. Nature, 387: 401–406.

Ahissar, M. & S. Hochstein 2004. The reverse hierarchy theory of visual perceptual learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8: 457–464.

Allan, M. D. 1958. Learning perceptual skills: the Sargeant system of recognition training. Occupational Psychology, 32: 245–252.

Anderson, J. R. & C. Lebiere 2003. The Newell Test for a theory of cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26: 587–601.

Archambault, A., C. O’Donnell & P. G. Schyns 1999. Blind to object changes: when learning the same object at different levels of categorization modifies its perception. Psychological Science, 10: 249–255.

Armstrong, D. 1997. A World of States of Affairs. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ashworth, A. R. S. & I. E. Dror 2000. Object identification as a function of discriminability and learning presentations: the effect of stimulus similarity and canonical frame alignment on aircraft identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6: 148–157.

Atkinson, J., F. Campbell & M. Francis 1976. The magic number 4 + 0: a new look at visual numerosity judgements. Perception, 5: 327–334.

Atran, S. 1998. Folk biology and the anthropology of science: cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21: 547–609.

Atran, S. 2002. Modular and cultural factors in biological understanding: an experimental approach to the cognitive basis of science. In P. Carruthers, S. Stich & M. Siegal (eds) The Cognitive Basis of Science, 41–72. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Atran, S., D. Medin & N. Ross 2004. Evolution and devolution of knowledge: a tale of two biologies. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 10: 395–420.

Aydede, M. 2000. On the type/token relation of mental representations. Facta Philosophica, 2: 23–49.

Baayen, R. H., R. Piepenbrock & L. Gulikers 1995. The CELEX lexical database [CD-ROM]. Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.

Bach, K. 2000. Review of Fodor (1998a). The Philosophical Review, 109: 627–632.

Baillargeon, R. 2002. The acquisition of physical knowledge in infancy: a summary in eight lessons. In P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart (eds) Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development, 47–83. Oxford: Blackwell.

Baker, L. R. 1991. Has content been naturalized? In Loewer & Rey (eds) 1991, 17–32.

Barrouillet, P., N. Grosset & J.-F. Lecas 2000. Conditional reasoning by mental models: chronometric and developmental evidence. Cognition, 75: 237–266.

Barrouillet, P. & J.-F. Lecas 1998. How can mental models theory account for content effects in conditional reasoning? A developmental perspective. Cognition, 67: 209–253.

Bartlett, J. C. & J. H. Searcy 1993. Inversion and configuration of faces. Cognitive Psychology, 25: 281–316.

Barwise, J. & J. Perry 1981. Situations and Attitudes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Berdoy, M., J. P. Webster & D. W. Macdonald 2000. Fatal attraction in rats infected with Toxoplasma gondii. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London (Series B: Biological Sciences), 267: 1591–1594.

Berlin, B. 1992. Ethnobiological Classification: Principles of Categorization of Plants and Animals in Traditional Societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Berlin, B., D. E. Breedlove & P. H. Raven 1973. General principles of classification and nomenclature in folk biology. American Anthropologist, 75: 214–242.

Berretty, P. M., P. M. Todd & L. Martignon 1999. Categorization by elimination: using few cues to choose. In Gigerenzer et al. 1999, 235–254.

Besner, D. & G. Humphreys (eds) 1991. Basic Processes in Reading: Visual Word Recognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Biederman, I. 1987. Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94: 115–147.

Biederman, I. & M. M. Shiffrar 1987. Sexing day-old chicks: a case study and expert systems analysis of a difficult perceptual-learning task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13: 640–645.

Blakemore, D. 2000. Indicators and procedures: ‘nevertheless’ and ‘but’. Journal of Linguistics, 36: 463–486.

Blanchette, P. A. 2001. Logical consequence. In L. Goble (ed.) The Blackwell Guide to Philosophical Logic, 115–135. Oxford: Blackwell.

Bliem, H. R. & A. Danek 1999. Direct evidence for a consistent dissociation between structural facial discrimination and facial individuation in prosopagnosia. Brain & Cognition, 40: 48–52.

Block, N. 1986. Advertisement for a semantics for psychology. In P. French, T. Uehling & H. Wettstein (eds.) Midwest Studies in Philosophy, Volume 10: Studies in the Philosophy of Mind, 615–678. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Block, N. 1993. Holism, hyper-analyticity and hyper-compositionality. Mind and Language, 8: 1–26.

Bloom, P. 1996. Intention, history, and artifact concepts. Cognition, 60: 1–29.

Bloom, P. 1998. Theories of artifact categorisation. Cognition, 66: 87–93.

Bloom, P. 2000. How Children Learn the Meanings of Words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Boghossian, P. A. 1991. Naturalizing content. In Loewer & Rey (eds) 1991, 65–86.

Boghossian, P. A. 1993. Does an inferential role semantics rest upon a mistake? Mind and Language, 8: 27–40.

Boghossian, P. A. 1994. Inferential role semantics and the analytic/synthetic distinction. Philosophical Studies, 73: 109–122.

Boghossian, P. A. 1997. Analyticity. In B. Hale & C. Wright (eds) A Companion to the Philosophy of Language, 331–368. Oxford: Blackwell.

Booth, A. E. & S. Waxman 2002. Object names and object functions serve as cues to categories for infants. Developmental Psychology, 38: 948–957.

Braine, M. D. S. & D. P. O’Brien 1991. A theory of if: A lexical entry, reasoning program, and pragmatic principles. Psychological Review, 98: 182–203.

Brandom, R. B. 1998. Insights and blindspots of reliabilism. The Monist, 81: 371–392.

Brook, A. & R. J. Stainton 1997. Fodor’s new theory of content and computation. Mind and Language, 12: 459–474.

Brown, R. 1958. How shall a thing be called? Psychological Review, 65: 14–21.

Burge, T. 1979. Individualism and the mental. In P. A. French, T. E. Uehling & H. K. Wettstein (eds) Studies in Metaphysics. Midwest Studies in Philosophy, vol. IV, 73–121. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Burton, A. M., V. Bruce & R. A. Johnston 1990. Understanding face recognition with an interactive activation model. British Journal of Psychology, 81: 361–380.

Bushnell, I. W. R. 1982. Discrimination of faces by young infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33: 298–308.

Cain, M. J. 2002. Fodor: Language, Mind and Philosophy. Cambridge, UK: Polity.

Caramazza, A. & J. R. Shelton 1998. Domain-specific knowledge systems in the brain: the animate-inanimate distinction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10: 1–34.

Carey, S. 1992. Becoming a face expert. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London (Series B: Biological Sciences), 335: 95–102.

Carey, S. & R. Diamond 1994. Are faces perceived as configurations more by adults than by children? Visual Cognition, 1: 253–274.

Carnap, R. 1952. Meaning postulates. Philosophical Studies, 3: 65–73.

Carnap, R. 1956. Meaning and Necessity, enlarged edition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Carroll, L. 1895. What the tortoise said to Achilles. Mind, 4: 278–280.

Carruthers, P., S. Laurence & S. Stich (eds) 2005. The Innate Mind: Structure and Content. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Carston, R. 1998. Informativeness, relevance and scalar implicature. In R. Carston & S. Uchida (eds) Relevance Theory: Applications and Implications, 179–236. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Carston, R. 2002. Thoughts and Utterances: The Pragmatics of Explicit Communication. Oxford: Blackwell.

Chance, J. E., A. L. Turner & A. G. Goldstein 1982. Development of differential recognition for own- and other-race faces. Journal of Psychology, 112: 29–37.

Chierchia, G. & S. McConnell-Ginet 2000. Meaning and Grammar, 2nd edition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Clark, H. H. 1969a. Linguistic processes in deductive reasoning. Psychological Review, 76: 387–404.

Clark, H. H. 1969b. The influence of language in solving three-term series problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 82: 205–215.

Clark, H. H. 1971. More about “adjectives, comparatives, and syllogisms”: a reply to Huttenlocher and Higgins. Psychological Review, 78: 505–514.

Cleeremans, A., A. Destrebecqz & M. Boyer 2000. Implicit learning: news from the front. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2: 406–416.

Coleman, L. & P. Kay 1981. Prototype semantics: the English word lie. Language, 57: 26–44.

Coltheart, M. 1980. Reading, phonological recoding and deep dyslexia. In M. Coltheart, K. E. Patterson & J. Marshall (eds) Deep Dyslexia, 197–226. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Coltheart, M. 2005. Modelling reading: the dual-route approach. In Snowling & Hulme (eds) 2005, 6–23.

Coltheart, M. & V. Coltheart 1997. Reading comprehension is not exclusively reliant upon phonological representation. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14: 167–175.

Cormack, A. 1998. Definitions: Implications for Syntax, Semantics, and the Language of Thought. New York: Garland.

Cosmides, L. & J. Tooby 2000a. Consider the source: the evolution of adaptations for decoupling and metarepresentation. In Sperber (ed.) 2000, 53–115.

Cosmides, L. & J. Tooby 2000b. Evolutionary psychology and the emotions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (eds) Handbook of Emotions, 2nd edition, 91–115. New York: Guilford.

Cosmides, L., J. Tooby & R. Kurzban 2003. Perceptions of race. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7: 173–179.

Cowie, F. 1999. What’s Within?: Nativism Reconsidered. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Crain, S., A. Gualmini & P. Pietroski 2005. Brass tacks in linguistic theory: innate grammatical principles. In Carruthers et al. (eds) 2005, 175–197.

Cresswell, M. J. 1978. Semantic competence. In M. Guenthner-Reutter & F. Guenthner (eds) Meaning and Translation, 9–28. London: Duckworth.

Currie, G. 2000. Imagination, delusion and hallucinations. Mind and Language, 15: 168–183.

de Almeida, R. G. 1999. What do category-specific semantic deficits tell us about the representation of lexical concepts? Brain and Language, 68: 241–248.

De Renzi, E. & F. Lucchelli 1994. Are semantic systems separately represented in the brain?: The case of living category impairment. Cortex, 30: 3–25.

de Waal, F. B. M. 1999a. The end of nature versus nurture. Scientific American, 281.6 (December 1999): 56–61.

de Waal, F. B. M. 1999b. Cultural primatology comes of age. Nature, 399: 635–636.

de Waal, F. B. M. 2001. The Ape and the Sushi Master. New York: Basic Books.

Dehaene, S. 1997. The Number Sense. New York: Oxford University Press.

Dennett, D. C. 1975. Brain writing and mind reading. In K. Gunderson (ed.) Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science, Volume VII: Language, Mind, and Knowledge, 403–416. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Dennett, D. C. 1990. The interpretation of texts, people and other artifacts. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 50 (Supplement): 177–194.

Diamond, R. & S. Carey 1986. Why faces are and are not special: an effect of expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115: 107–117.

Dowty, D. 1979. Word Meaning and Montague Grammar. Dordrecht: Reidel.

Dretske, F. 1977. Laws of nature. Philosophy of Science, 44: 248–268.

Dretske, F. 1981. Knowledge and the Flow of Information. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Dretske, F. 1983. Précis of Knowledge and the Flow of Information. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6: 55–90. (Includes commentaries and author’s response)

Dretske, F. 1986. Misrepresentation. In R. Bogdan (ed.) Belief, 17–36. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Dretske, F. 1989. Reasons and causes. Philosophical Perspectives, 3: 1–15.

Drewery, A. 2005. Essentialism and the necessity of the laws of nature. Synthese, 144: 381–396.

Dreyfus, H. L. 1990. Socratic and platonic sources of cognitivism. In J.-C. Smith (ed.) Historical Foundations of Cognitive Science, 1–17. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

Ekman, P., M. O’Sullivan & M. G. Frank 1999. A few can catch a liar. Psychological Science, 10: 263–266.

Ellis, H. D., D. M. Jones & N. Mosdell 1997. Intra- and inter-modal repetition priming of familiar faces and voices. British Journal of Psychology, 88: 143–156.

Epstein, S. 1994. Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49: 709–724.

Epstein, S., A. Lipson, C. Holstein & E. Huh 1992. Irrational reactions to negative outcomes: evidence for two conceptual systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62: 328–339.

Ericsson, K. A., R. T. Krampe & C. Tesch-Römer 1993. The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100: 363–406.

Ericsson, K. A. & A. C. Lehmann 1996. Expert and exceptional performance: evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47: 273–305.

Evans, J. St. B. T., S. J. Handley & D. E. Over 2003. Conditionals and conditional probability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29: 321–335.

Evans, J. St. B. T., S. E. Newstead & R. M. J. Byrne 1993. Human Reasoning: The Psychology of Deduction. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Evans, J. St. B. T. & D. E. Over 2004. If. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Everett, A. 2005. Recent defences of descriptivism. Mind & Language, 20: 103–139.

Fabre-Thorpe, M., A. Delorme, C. Marlot & S. Thorpe 2001. A limit to the speed of processing in ultra-rapid visual categorization of novel natural scenes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13: 171–180.

Farah, M. J. 2004. Visual Agnosia, 2nd edition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Farah, M. J., C. Rabinowitz, G. E. Quinn & G. T. Liu 2000. Early commitment of neural substrates for face recognition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17: 117–123.

Farah, M. J., K. D. Wilson, H. M. Drain & J. R. Tanaka 1995. The inverted face inversion effect in prosopagnosia: evidence for mandatory, face-specific perceptual mechanisms. Vision Research, 35: 2089–2093.

Farah, M. J., K. D. Wilson, M. Drain & J. N. Tanaka 1998. What is “special” about face perception? Psychological Review, 105: 482–498.

Field, H. 1977. Logic, meaning and conceptual role. Journal of Philosophy, 74: 379–409.

Fine, K. 1982. First-order modal theories. III: Facts. Synthese, 53: 43–122.

Fodor, J. A. 1970. Three reasons for not deriving “kill” from “cause to die”. Linguistic Inquiry, 1: 429–438.

Fodor, J. A. 1975. The Language of Thought. New York: Crowell.

Fodor, J. A. 1978. Propositional attitudes. In Representations: Philosophical Essays on the Foundations of Cognitive Science, 177–203. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Fodor, J. A. 1980. Fixation of belief and concept acquisition. In M. Piattelli-Palmarini (ed.) Language and Learning: The Debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky, 143–149. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Fodor, J. A. 1981. The present status of the innateness controversy. In Representations: Philosophical Essays on the Foundations of Cognitive Science, 257–316. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Fodor, J. A. 1982. Cognitive science and the Twin-Earth problem. Notre Dame Journal of Formal Logic, 23: 98–118.

Fodor, J. A. 1983. The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Fodor, J. A. 1984a. Semantics, Wisconsin style. Synthese, 59: 231–250.

Fodor, J. A. 1984b/1990. Psychosemantics or: Where do truth conditions come from? Ms., MIT. (Published in W. G. Lycan (ed.) Mind and Cognition, 312–337. Oxford: Blackwell, 1990.)

Fodor, J. A. 1984c. Observation reconsidered. Philosophy of Science, 51: 23–43.

Fodor, J. A. 1987. Psychosemantics: The Problem of Meaning in the Philosophy of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Fodor, J. A. 1990a. A theory of content, I: The problem. In A Theory of Content and Other Essays, 51–87. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Fodor, J. A. 1990b. A theory of content, II: The theory. In A Theory of Content and Other Essays, 89–136. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Fodor, J. A. 1990c. Information and representation. In P. Hanson (ed.) Information, Language, and Cognition, 175–190. Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press.

Fodor, J. A. 1991. A modal argument for narrow content. Journal of Philosophy, 88: 5–26.

Fodor, J. A. 1994. The Elm and the Expert: Mentalese and its Semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Fodor, J. A. 1998a. Concepts: Where Cognitive Science Went Wrong. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Fodor, J. A. 1998b. There are no recognitional concepts—not even RED. Philosophical Issues, 9: 1–14.

Fodor, J. A. 2000a. Replies to critics. Mind and Language, 15: 350–374.

Fodor, J. A. 2000b. The Mind Doesn’t Work That Way: The Scope and Limits of Computational Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Fodor, J. A. 2001a. Doing without What’s Within: Fiona Cowie’s critique of nativism. Mind, 107: 99–148.

Fodor, J. A. 2001b. Language, thought and compositionality. Mind and Language, 16: 1–15.

Fodor, J. A. 2003a. Hume Variations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Fodor, J. A. 2003b. Crossed wires: the link between what’s in the genes and what’s in the mind. Review of Ridley (2003). Times Literary Supplement, 16 May 2003: 3–4.

Fodor, J. A. 2004a. Having concepts: a brief refutation of the twentieth century. Mind and Language, 19: 29–47.

Fodor, J. A. 2004b. Reply to commentators. Mind and Language, 19: 99–112.

Fodor, J. A. 2004c. Revenge of the given. Ms., Rutgers.

Fodor, J. A., M. F. Garrett, E. C. T. Walker & C. H. Parkes 1980. Against definitions. Cognition, 8: 263–367.

Fodor, J. A. & E. Lepore 1991. Why meaning (probably) isn’t conceptual role. Mind and Language, 6: 328–343.

Fodor, J. A. & E. Lepore 1992. Holism: A Shopper’s Guide. Oxford: Blackwell.

Fodor, J. A. & E. Lepore 2002. The Compositionality Papers. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Fodor, J. A. & E. Lepore 2005. Out of context. Proceedings and Addresses of the American Philosophical Association, 78: 3–20.

Fodor, J. A. & Z. W. Pylyshyn 1981. How direct is visual perception?: Some reflections on Gibson’s “Ecological Approach”. Cognition, 9: 139–196.

Fodor, J. D. 1977. Semantics: Theories of Meaning in Generative Grammar. New York: Crowell.

Fodor, J. D., J. A. Fodor & M. F. Garrett 1975. The psychological unreality of semantic representations. Linguistic Inquiry, 4: 515–531.

Galef, B. G. 1988. Imitation in animals. In T. R. Sentall & B. G. Galef (eds) Social Learning: Psychological and Biological Perspectives, 3–28. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Garcia, J. & R. A. Koelling 1966. The relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science, 4: 123–124.

Gates, G. 1996. The price of information. Synthese, 107: 325–347.

Gauthier, I., T. Curran, K. M. Curby & D. Collins 2003. Perceptual interference supports a non-modular account of face processing. Nature Neuroscience, 6: 428–432.

Gauthier, I. & C. A. Nelson 2001. The development of face expertise. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11: 219–224.

Gauthier, I., P. Skudlarski, J. C. Gore & A. W. Anderson 2000. Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition. Nature Neuroscience, 3: 191–197.

Gauthier, I. & M. J. Tarr 1997. Becoming a “Greeble” expert: exploring mechanisms for face recognition. Vision Research, 37: 1673–1682.

Gauthier, I., P. Williams, M. J. Tarr & J. Tanaka 1998. Training “Greeble” experts: a framework for studying expert object recognition processes. Vision Research, 38: 2401–2428.

Gazdar, G. & G. K. Pullum 1976. Truth-functional connectives in natural language. In S. S. Mufwene, C. A. Walker & S. B. Steever (eds) Papers from the Twelfth Regional Meeting, Chicago Linguistic Society, 220–234. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.

Gelman, R., F. Durgin & L. Kaufman 1995. Distinguishing between animates and inanimates: not by motion alone. In D. Sperber, D. Premack & A. J. Premack (eds) Causal Cognition: A Multidisciplinary Debate, 150–184. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Gentzen, G. 1935/1969. Investigations into logical deduction. In M. E. Szabo (ed.) The Collected Papers of Gerhard Gentzen, 68–131. Amsterdam: North Holland.

Geurts, B. 1997. Good news about the description theory of names. Journal of Semantics, 14: 319–348.

Gibson, E. J. 1969. Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Gibson, E. J., A. Pick, H. Osser & M. Hammond 1962. The role of grapheme-phoneme correspondence in the perception of words. American Journal of Psychology, 75: 554–570.

Gibson, E. J. & R. D. Walk 1960. The “visual cliff”. Scientific American, 202.4 (April 1960): 64–71.

Gibson, J. J. 1948. The Possibilities of Research on Training in Recognition and other Visual Performances. Report to Armed Forces by the NRC Vision Committee, University of Michigan.

Gibson, J. J. 1966. The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Gibson, J. J. 1979. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Gibson, J. J. & R. M. Gagné 1947. Research on the recognition of aircraft. In J. J. Gibson (ed.) Motion Picture Testing and Research, 113–168. (Army Air Force Aviation Psychology Program Research Report No. 7.) Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

Gigerenzer, G., P. M. Todd & The ABC Research Group 1999. Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Gilbert, C. D. & T. N. Wiesel 1992. Receptive field dynamics in adult primary visual cortex. Nature, 356: 150–152.

Goddard, C. & A. Wierzbicka (eds) 2002. Meaning and Universal Grammar: Theory and Empirical Findings (2 volumes). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Goldstone, R. L. 1994. Influences of categorization on perceptual discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123: 178–200.

Goldstone, R. L. 1998. Perceptual learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 49: 585–612.

Goodman, N. 1976. Languages of Art, 2nd edition. Indianapolis: Hackett.

Gould, J. L. 2002. Learning instincts. In H. Pashler & R. Gallistel (eds) Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology, vol. 3, 3rd edition, 239–257. New York: Wiley.

Gould, J. L. & P. Marler 1984. Ethology and the natural history of learning. In P. Marler & H. Terrace (eds) The Biology of Learning, 47–74. Berlin: Springer.

Gould, J. L. & P. Marler 1987. Learning by instinct. Scientific American, 256.1 (January 1987): 74–85.

Grice, H. P. 1957. Meaning. Philosophical Review, 66: 377–88.

Grice, H. P. 1967. Logic and Conversation. William James Lectures, reprinted in Grice 1989, 1–143.

Grice, H. P. 1989. Studies in the Way of Words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Gunther, Y. H. (ed.) 2003. Essays on Nonconceptual Content. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Hadjichristidis, C., R. J. Stevenson, D. E. Over, S. A. Sloman, J. St. B. T. Evans & A. Feeney 2001. On the evaluation of ‘if p then q’ conditionals. In J. D. Moore & K. Stenning (eds) Proceedings of the Twenty-Third Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 409–414. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Haig, N. D. 1984. The effect of feature displacement on face recognition. Perception, 13: 505–512.

Hancock, P. J. B., V. Bruce & A. M. Burton 2000. Recognition of unfamiliar faces. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4: 330–337.

Hanson, W. H. 1997. The concept of logical consequence. The Philosophical Review, 106: 365–409.

Harman, G. H. 2002. Internal critique of logic and practical reasoning. In D. M. Gabbay & H. J. Ohlbach (eds) Studies in Logic and Practical Reasoning, vol. I, 171–186. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Harnad, S. 1996. Experimental analysis of naming behavior cannot explain naming capacity. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65: 262–264.

Heiling, A. M. & M. E. Herberstein 1999. The role of experience in web-building spiders (Araneidae). Animal Cognition, 2: 171–177.

Hermer, L. & E. Spelke 1996. Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation. Cognition, 61: 195–232.

Hillis, A. & A. Caramazza 1991. Category-specific naming and comprehension impairment: a double dissociation. Brain, 114: 2081–2094.

Hirschfeld, L. 2001. On a folk theory of society: children, evolution, and mental representations of social groups. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5: 107–117.

Hochstein, S. & M. Ahissar 2002. View from the top: hierarchies and reverse hierarchies in the visual system. Neuron, 36: 791–804.

Hoffman, D. D. & M. Singh 1997. Salience of visual parts. Cognition, 63: 29–78.

Horton, M. S. & E. M. Markman 1980. Developmental differences in the acquisition of basic and superordinate categories. Child Development, 51: 708–719.

Horwich, P. 1992. Chomsky versus Quine on the analytic–synthetic distinction. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 92: 95–108.

Horwich, P. 1998. Meaning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Hubel, D. H. & T. N. Wiesel 1962. Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. Journal of Physiology (London), 160: 106–154.

Hubel, D. H. & T. N. Wiesel 1968. Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. Journal of Physiology (London), 195: 215–243.

Hugson, A. & R. Boakes 2001. Perceptual and cognitive elements of wine tasting expertise. Australian Journal of Psychology, 53: 103–108.

Hurford, J. R. 1974. Exclusive or inclusive disjunction. Foundations of Language, 11: 409–411.

Intraub, H. 1981. Rapid conceptual identification of sequentially presented pictures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7: 604–610.

Jackendoff, R. 1992. Languages of the Mind: Essays on Mental Representation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Jackson, F. 1998. From Metaphysics to Ethics: A Defence of Conceptual Analysis. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Jaggi, S., W. C. Karl, S. G. Mallat & A. S. Willsky 1999. Silhouette recognition using high-resolution pursuit. Pattern Recognition, 32: 753–771.

James, W. 1890. The Principles of Psychology, vol. 1. New York: Holt.

Jennings, R. E. 1994. The Genealogy of Disjunction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Jiang, Y. & M. M. Chun 2001. Selective attention modulates implicit learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 54A: 1105–1124.

Johnson, K. E. 1992. The Effect of Expertise on Hierarchical Systems of Categorization. Ph.D. Dissertation, Emory University, Atlanta.

Johnson, K. E. & C. B. Mervis 1997. Effects of varying levels of expertise on the basic level of categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126: 248–277.

Johnson-Laird, P. N. 1993. Human and Machine Thinking. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Johnson-Laird, P. N. 2005. If bears eat in the woods…? Review of Evans & Over (2004). Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9: 43–44.

Johnson-Laird, P. N. & J. Tagart 1969. How implication is understood. American Journal of Psychology, 82: 367–373.

Jones, S. 1970. Visual and verbal processes in problem-solving. Cognitive Psychology, 1: 201–214.

Jones, S. S. & L. B. Smith 2002. How children know the relevant properties for generalizing object names. Developmental Science, 5: 219–232.

Jones, S. S., L. B. Smith & B. Landau 1991. Object properties and knowledge in early lexical learning. Child Development, 62: 499–516.

Jubien, M. 1989. On properties and property theory. In G. Chierchia, B. H. Partee & R. Turner (eds) Properties, Types and Meaning, vol. 1, 159–175. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

Kalderon, M. E. 2001. Reasoning and representing. Philosophical Studies, 105: 129–160.

Karni, A. & D. Sagi 1993. The time course of learning a visual skill. Nature, 365: 250–252.

Katz, J. 1972. Semantic Theory. New York: Harper & Row.

Keil, F. C. & R. A. Wilson 2000. The concept concept: the wayward path of cognitive science. Mind and Language, 15: 308–318.

Klahr, D. 1973. A production system for counting, subitizing and adding. In W. G. Chase (ed.) Visual Information Processing, 527–546. New York: Academic Press.

Kneale, W. 1962. Modality de dicto and de re. In E. Nagel, P. Suppes & A. Tarski (eds) Logic, Methodology and Philosophy of Science. Proceedings of the 1960 International Congress, 622–633. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

Kobatake, E. & K. Tanaka 1994. Neuronal selectivities to complex object features in the ventral visual pathway of the macaque cerebral cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 71: 856–867.

Koslow, A. 1992. A Structuralist Theory of Logic. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Kreiman, G., C. Koch & I. Fried 2000. Category-specific visual responses of single neurons in the human medial temporal lobe. Nature Neuroscience, 3: 946–953.

Kripke, S. 1972/1980. Naming and Necessity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Lakoff, G. & J. R. Ross 1976. Is deep structure necessary? In J. D. McCawley (ed.) Syntax and Semantics, vol. 7: Notes from the Linguistic Underground, 159–164. New York: Academic Press.

Lakoff, R. 1971. If’s, and’s and but’s about conjunction. In C. J. Fillmore & D. T. Langendoen (eds) Studies in Linguistic Semantics, 114–149. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Landau, B. 1994. Object shape, object name, and object kind. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 31: 253–304.

Landau, B. 2000. Concepts, the lexicon and acquisition: Fodor’s new challenge. Mind and Language, 15: 319–326.

Landau, B., L. Smith & S. Jones 1998a. Object perception and object naming in early development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2: 19–24.

Landau, B., L. Smith & S. Jones 1998b. Object shape, object function, and object naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 38: 1–27.

Laurence, S. & E. Margolis 1999a. Concepts and cognitive science. In Margolis & Laurence (eds) Concepts: Core Readings, 3–81. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Laurence, S. & E. Margolis 1999b. Review of Fodor (1998a). British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 50: 487–491.

Laurence, S. & E. Margolis 2003a. Should we trust our intuitions? Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 103: 299–323.

Laurence, S. & E. Margolis 2003b. Concepts. In S. P. Stich & T. Warfield (eds) The Blackwell Guide to the Philosophy of Mind, 190–213. Oxford, Blackwell.

Lepore, E. 2000. Meaning and Argument. Oxford: Blackwell.

Leslie, A. M. 1994. Pretending and believing: issues in the theory of ToMM. Cognition, 50: 211–238.

Leslie, A. M., O. Friedman & T. P. German 2004. Core mechanisms in ‘theory of mind’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8: 528–533.

Leslie, A. M. & S. Keeble 1987. Do 6-month old infants perceive causality? Cognition, 25: 265–288.

Levine, A. & M. H. Bickhard 1999. Concepts: Where Fodor went wrong. Philosophical Psychology, 12: 5–23.

Lewicki, P., T. Hill & M. Czyzewska 1992. Nonconscious acquisition of information. American Psychologist, 47: 796–801.

Lieberman, M. D., R. Gaunt, D. T. Gilbert & Y. Trope 2002. Reflexion and reflection: a social cognitive neuroscience approach to attributional inference. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34: 199–249.

Loewer, B. & G. Rey (eds) 1991. Meaning in Mind: Fodor and his Critics. Oxford: Blackwell.

Łukasiewicz, J. 1920/1970. On three-valued logic. In Łukasiewicz 1970, 87–88.

Łukasiewicz, J. 1970. Selected Works (ed. L. Borkowski). Amsterdam: North Holland.

Lukatela, G. & M. T. Turvey 1991. Phonological access of the lexicon: evidence from associative priming with pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17: 951–966.

Lukatela, G. & M. T. Turvey 1993. Similar attentional, frequency, and associative effects for pseudohomophones and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19: 166–178.

Lukatela, G. & M. T. Turvey 1994a. Visual lexical access is initially phonological: 1. Evidence from associative priming by words, homophones, and pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123: 107–128.

Lukatela, G. & M. T. Turvey 1994b. Visual lexical access is initially phonological: 2. Evidence from phonological priming by homophones and pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123: 331–353.

Lunn, J. H. 1948. Chick sexing. American Scientist, 36: 280–287.

Lycan, W. 1984. Logical Form in Natural Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Lyons, J. 1977. Semantics (2 volumes). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

McCandliss, B. D., L. Cohen & S. Dehaene 2003. The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7: 293–299.

McCawley, J. D. 1968. Lexical insertion in a transformational grammar without deep structure. Papers from the Fourth Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, 71–80. Reprinted in J. D. McCawley 1976. Grammar and Meaning, 155–166. New York: Academic Press.

McGinn, C. 1989. Mental Content. Oxford: Blackwell.

Mack, A. 2003. Inattentional blindness: looking without seeing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12: 180–184.

Mack, A. & I. Rock 1998. Inattentional Blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Madole, K. L. & L. B. Cohen 1995. The role of object parts in infants’ attention to form–function correlations. Developmental Psychology, 31: 637–648.

Malt, B. C. & E. C. Johnson 1998. Artifact category membership and the intentional-historical theory. Cognition, 66: 79–85.

Margolis, E. 1998. How to acquire a concept. Mind and Language, 13: 347–369.

Marler, P. 1991. The instinct to learn. In S. Carey & R. Gelman (eds) The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition, 37–66. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Marler, P. 2004. Innateness and the instinct to learn. Anais da Academia Brasileira de Ciências, 76: 189–200.

Marr, D. 1982. Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information. San Francisco: Freeman.

Martin, J. N. 1987. Elements of Formal Semantics. London: Academic Press.

Martin, R. D. 1994. The Specialist Chick Sexer: A History, A World View, Future Prospects. Melbourne: Bernal Publishing.

Masui, K. & J. Hashimoto 1933. Sexing Baby Chicks. Vancouver: Journal Printing Company.

Matan, A. & S. Carey 2001. Developmental changes within the core of artifact concepts. Cognition, 78: 1–26.

Maye, J., J. F. Werker & L. Gerken 2002. Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82: B101–B111.

Miller, G. A. 1978. Semantic relations among words. In M. Halle, J. Bresnan & G. Miller (eds) Linguistic Theory and Psychological Reality, 60–118. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Millikan, R. G. 1984. Language, Thought, and Other Biological Categories: New Foundations for Realism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Millikan, R. G. 1993. On Mentalese orthography. In B. Dahlbom (ed.) Dennett and his Critics: Demystifying Mind, 97–123. Oxford: Blackwell.

Milne, P. 1994. Classical harmony: rules of inference and the meaning of the logical constants. Synthese, 100: 49–94.

Morton, J. & M. H. Johnson 1991. CONSPEC and CONLERN: a two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98: 164–181.

Murphy, G. L. 2002. The Big Book of Concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Musgrave, A. 1993. Common Sense, Science and Scepticism: A Historical Introduction to the Theory of Knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Myers, D. G. 2002. Intuition: Its Powers and Perils. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Nelson, C. A. 2001. The development and neural bases of face recognition. Infant and Child Development, 10: 3–18.

Nelson, M. 2002. Descriptivism defended. Noûs, 36: 408–436.

Neuner, F. & S. R. Schweinberger 2000. Neuropsychological impairments in the recognition of faces, voices, and personal names. Brain and Cognition, 44: 342–366.

Nichols, S. & S. Stich 2000. A cognitive theory of pretense. Cognition, 74: 115–147.

Noveck, I. A. 2004. Pragmatic inferences linked to logical terms. In I. A. Noveck & D. Sperber (eds) Experimental Pragmatics, 301–321. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.

Opfer, J. E. & R. S. Siegler 2004. Revisiting preschoolers’ living things concept: a microgenetic analysis of conceptual change in basic biology. Cognitive Psychology, 49: 301–332.

Over, D. E. & J. St. B. T. Evans 2003. The probability of conditionals: The psychological evidence. Mind & Language, 18: 340–358.

Palmeri, T. J. 1997. Exemplar similarity and the development of automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23: 324–354.

Partee, B. H., A. ter Meulen & R. E. Wall 1993. Mathematical Methods in Linguistics. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

Pascalis, O., E. Demont, M. de Haan & R. Campbell 2001. Recognition of faces of different species: a developmental study between 5 and 8 years of age. Infant and Child Development, 10: 39–45.

Pascalis, O., S. de Schonen, J. Morton, C. Deruelle & M. Fabre-Grenet 1995. Mother’s face recognition by neonates: a replication and an extension. Infant Behavior and Development, 18: 79–85.

Patel, V. L., J. F. Arocha & D. R. Kaufmann 1994. Diagnostic reasoning and medical expertise. In D. Medin (ed.) The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, vol. 31, 187–251. New York: Academic Press.

Pavlov, I. 1927. Conditioned Reflexes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Peacocke, C. 1983. Sense and Content. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Peacocke, C. 1992. A Study of Concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Peacocke, C. 2004. Interrelations: Concepts, knowledge, reference and structure. Mind and Language, 19: 85–98.

Peirce, C. S. 1931–1935. Collected Papers, vol. II. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Pelletier, F. J. 1977. Or. Theoretical Linguistics, 4: 61–74.

Perner, J. & B. Lang 2000. Theory of mind and executive function: is there a developmental relationship? In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg & D. J.

Cohen (eds) Understanding Other Minds, 2nd edition (Perspectives from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience), 150–181. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Piattelli-Palmarini, M. 1989. Evolution, selection and cognition: from “learning” to parameter setting in biology and in the study of language. Cognition, 31: 1–44.

Premack, D. 1990. The infant’s theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition, 36: 1–16.

Premack, D. & A. J. Premack 1995. Intention as psychological cause. In D. Sperber, D. Premack & A. J. Premack (eds) Causal Cognition: A Multidisciplinary Debate, 185–199. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Prinz, J. & A. Clark 2004. Putting concepts to work: some thoughts for the twenty-first century. Mind and Language, 19: 57–69.

Prior, A. N. 1960. The runabout inference-ticket. Analysis, 21: 38–39.

Pullum, G. K. 1991. The great Eskimo vocabulary hoax. In The Great Eskimo Vocabulary Hoax and Other Irreverent Essays on the Study of Language, 159–171. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Putnam, H. 1975. The meaning of “meaning”. In K. Gunderson (ed.) Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science, Volume VII: Language, Mind, and Knowledge, 131–193. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Putnam, H. 1983. ‘Two dogmas’ revisited. In Realism and Reason (Philosophical Papers, vol. 3), 87–97. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Pylyshyn, Z. W. 2000. Situating vision in the world. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4: 197–207.

Pyysiäinen, I. 2003. Dual-process theories and hybrid systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26: 617–618. (Commentary on Anderson & Lebiere 2003.)

Quine, W. V. O. 1935/1976. Truth by convention. In Quine 1976, 77–106.

Quine, W. V. O. 1948/1953. On what there is. In Quine 1953b, 1–19.

Quine, W. V. O. 1953a. Two dogmas of empiricism. In Quine 1953b, 20–46

Quine, W. V. O. 1953b. From a Logical Point of View. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Quine, W. V. O. 1954/1976. Carnap and logical truth. In Quine 1976, 107–132.

Quine, W. V. O. 1960. Word and Object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Quine, W. V. O. 1973/1976. Vagaries of definition. In Quine 1976, 50–55.

Quine, W. V. O. 1976. The Ways of Paradox and Other Essays, revised and enlarged edition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Quine, W. V. O. 1977. Natural kinds. In S. P. Schwartz (ed.) Naming, Necessity, and Natural Kinds, 155–175. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

Rakison, D. H. & G. E. Butterworth 1998. Infants’ use of object parts in early categorization. Developmental Psychology, 34: 49–62.

Ray, G. 1997. Fodor and the inscrutability problem. Mind and Language, 12: 475–489.

Rayner, K., B. R. Foorman, C. A. Perfetti, D. Pesetsky & M. S. Seidenberg 2002. How should reading be taught? Scientific American, 286.3 (March 2002): 70–77.

Reed, C. F., P. N. Witt, M. B. Scarboro & D. B. Peakall 1970. Experience and the orb web. Developmental Psychobiology, 3: 251–265.

Reichenbach, H. 1944. Philosophical Foundations of Quantum Mechanics. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Rhodes, G. 1988. Looking at faces: first-order and second-order features as determinants of facial appearance. Perception, 17: 43–63.

Ridley, M. 2003. Nature Via Nurture: Genes, Experience, and What Makes Us Human. London: Fourth Estate.

Rips, L. 1983. Cognitive processes in propositional reasoning. Psychological Review, 90: 38–71.

Robbins, R. & E. McKone 2003. Can holistic processing be learned for inverted faces? Cognition, 88: 79–107.

Rosch, E., C. B. Mervis, W. Gray, D. Johnson & P. Boyes-Braem 1976. Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8: 382–439.

Rossion, B., L. Dricot, A. Devolder, J.-M. Bodart, M. Crommelinck, B. De Gelder & R. Zoontjes 2000. Hemispheric asymmetries for whole-based and part-based face processing in the human fusiform gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12: 793–802.

Sacchett, C. & G. W. Humphreys 1992. Calling a squirrel a squirrel but a canoe a wigwam: a category-specific deficit for artefactual objects and body parts. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9: 73–86.

Samuels, R. 2002. Nativism in cognitive science. Mind and Language, 17: 233–265.

Samuels, R. 2004. Innateness in cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8: 136–141.

Samuels, R. 2005. The complexity of cognition: tractability arguments for massive modularity. In Carruthers et al. (eds) 2005, 107–121.

Samuels, R. & S. Stich 2004. Rationality and psychology. In A. R. Mele & P. Rawling (eds) The Oxford Handbook of Rationality, 279–300. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Sapolsky, R. 2003. Bugs in the brain. Scientific American, 288.3: 94–97 (March 2003).

Schiffer, S. 1981. Truth and the theory of content. In H. Parret & J. Bouvaresse (eds) Meaning and Understanding, 204–222. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.

Schneider, W. & J. M. Chein 2003. Controlled & automatic processing: behavior, theory, and biological mechanisms. Cognitive Science, 27: 525–559.

Schyns, P. G., R. L. Goldstone & J.-P. Thibaut 1998. The development of features in object concepts. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21: 1–54.

Schyns, P. G. & G. L. Murphy 1994. The ontogeny of part representation in object concepts. In D. Medin (ed.) The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, vol. 31, 305–354. New York: Academic Press.

Schyns, P. G. & L. Rodet 1997. Categorization creates functional features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23: 681–696.

Scott, S. 2002. The psychological implausibility of naturalized content. In W. D. Gray & C. D. Schunn (eds) Proceedings of the Twenty-Fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 822–827. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Scott, S. 2003. Non-Referring Concepts. Ph.D. Dissertation, Carleton University, Ottawa. [Carleton University Cognitive Science Technical Report 2003-03. URL = <http://www.carleton.ca/iis/TechReports>]

Searcy, J. H. & J. C. Bartlett 1996. Inversion and processing of component and spatial-relational information in faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22: 904–915.

Segal, G. M. A. 2000. A Slim Book about Narrow Content. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Sergent, J. 1984. An investigation into component and configural processes underlying face perception. British Journal of Psychology, 75: 221–242.

Sewards, T. V. & M. A. Sewards 2002. Innate visual object recognition in vertebrates: some proposed pathways and mechanisms. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A, 132: 861–891.

Shastri, L. & V. Ajjanagadde 1993. From simple associations to systematic reasoning: a connectionist representation of rules, variables and dynamic bindings using temporal synchrony. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16: 417–494.

Sher, G. 1991. The Bounds of Logic: A Generalized Viewpoint. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Sher, G. 1996. Semantics and Logic. In S. Lappin (ed.) The Handbook of Contemporary Semantic Theory, 511–537. Oxford: Blackwell.

Simon, H. A. 1992. What is an “explanation” of behavior? Psychological Science, 3: 150–161.

Simon, H. A. & W. G. Chase 1973. Skill in chess. American Scientist, 61: 394–403.

Simons, D. J. & C. F. Chabris 1999. Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28: 1059–1074.

Sloman, S. A. 1996. The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119: 3–22.

Smith, E. R. & J. DeCoster 2000. Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4: 108–131.

Snowling, M. J. & C. Hulme (eds) 2005. The Science of Reading: A Handbook. Oxford: Blackwell.

Sober, E. 1982. Why logically equivalent predicates may pick out different properties. American Philosophical Quarterly, 19: 183–189.

Solomon, G. E. A. 1990. Psychology of novice and expert wine talk. American Journal of Psychology, 103: 495–517.

Sosa, D. 2001. Rigidity in the scope of Russell’s theory. Noûs, 35: 1–38.

Sperber, D. 1975. Rethinking Symbolism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Sperber, D. 1994. The modularity of thought and the epidemiology of representations. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (eds) Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture, 39–67. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Sperber, D. 1996. Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach. Oxford: Blackwell.

Sperber, D. 1997. Intuitive and reflective beliefs. Mind and Language, 12: 67–83.

Sperber, D. (ed.) 2000. Metarepresentations: A Multidisciplinary Perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Sperber, D. 2005. Modularity and relevance: How can a massively modular mind be flexible and context-sensitive? In Carruthers et al. (eds) 2005, 53–68.

Sperber, D., F. Cara & V. Girotto 1995. Relevance theory explains the selection task. Cognition, 52: 3–22.

Sperber, D. & D. Wilson 1983. Draft of Relevance. Ms., UCL.

Sperber, D. & D. Wilson 1995. Relevance, 2nd edition. Oxford: Blackwell.

Sperber, D. & D. Wilson 1998. The mapping between the mental and the public lexicon. In P. Carruthers & J. Boucher (eds) Language and Thought, 184–200. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Sperber, D. & D. Wilson 2002. Pragmatics, modularity and mind-reading. Mind and Language, 17: 3–23.

Stainton, R. J. & C. Viger 2000. Review of Fodor (1998a). Synthese, 123: 131–151.Stalnaker, R. 1998. What might nonconceptual content be? Philosophical Issues, 9: 339–352. Reprinted in Gunther 2003, 95–106.

Stanley, J. 1997. Names and rigid designation. In B. Hale & C. Wright (eds) A Companion to the Philosophy of Language, 555–585. Oxford: Blackwell.

Stich, S. P. 1978. Beliefs and subdoxastic states. Philosophy of Science, 45: 499–518.

Stroop, J. R. 1935. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12: 242–248.

Swoyer, C. 1996. Theories of properties: from plenitude to paucity. Philosophical Perspectives, 10: 243–264 (Supplement to Noûs, volume 30).

Swoyer, C. 2000. Properties. In E. N. Zalta (ed.) The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Winter 2000 Edition), URL = <http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2000/entries/properties/>.

Tanaka, J. W. & T. Curran 2001. A neural basis for expert object recognition. Psychological Science, 12: 43–47.

Tanaka, J. W., T. Curran & D. L. Sheinberg 2005. The training and transfer of real-world perceptual expertise. Psychological Science, 16: 145–151.

Tanaka, J. W. & M. J. Farah 1993. Parts and wholes in face recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 46A: 225–245.

Tanaka, J. W., M. Kiefer & C. Bukach 2004. A holistic account of the own-race effect in face recognition: evidence from a cross-cultural study. Cognition, 93: B1–B9.

Tanaka, J. W. & J. A. Sengco 1997. Features and their configuration in face recognition. Memory & Cognition, 25: 583–592.

Tanaka, J. W. & M. Taylor 1991. Object categories and expertise: is the basic level in the eye of the beholder? Cognitive Psychology, 23: 457–82.

Tarski, A. 1936/1956. On the concept of logical consequence. In Tarski 1956, 409–420.

Tarski, A. 1956. Logic, Semantics, Metamathematics (translated by J. H. Woodger). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Tarski, A. 1966/1986. What are logical notions? History and Philosophy of Logic, 7: 143–154. (Text of a 1966 lecture, edited by J. Corcoran.)

Tennant, N. 1987. Anti-realism and Logic. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Thorpe, S., D. Fize & C. Marlot 1996. Speed of processing in the human visual system. Nature, 381: 520–522.

Tinbergen, N. 1951. The Study of Instinct. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Tooby, J., L. Cosmides & C. Barrett 2005. Resolving the debate on innate ideas: Learnability constraints and the evolved interpenetration of motivational and conceptual functions. In Carruthers et al. (eds) 2005, 305–337.

Treisman, A. & G. Gelade 1980. A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12: 97–136.

Trick, L. M. & Z. W. Pylyshyn 1993. What enumeration studies can show us about spatial attention: evidence for limited capacity preattentive processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19: 331–351.

Tulving, E. 1972. Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (eds) Organization of Memory, 381–403. New York: Academic Press.

Tversky, A. 1972. Elimination by aspects: a theory of choice. Psychological Review, 79: 281–299.

United States Army. 1996. Visual Aircraft Recognition. Field Manual No. 44-80 (Rev. 30 September 1996). Washington, DC: Department of the Army.

Vallentyne, P. 1998. The nomic role account of carving reality at the joints. Synthese, 115: 171–198.

Van Dijk, T. A. 1977. Text and Context: Explorations in the Semantics and Pragmatics of Discourse. London: Longman.

Van Orden, G. C. 1987. A ROWS is a ROSE: spelling, sound, and reading. Memory & Cognition, 15: 181–198.

Van Orden, G. C. 1991. Phonologic mediation is fundamental to reading. In Besner & Humphreys (eds) 1991, 77–103.

Van Orden, G. C., J. C. Johnston & B. L. Hale 1988. Word identification in reading proceeds from spelling to sound to meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14: 371–386.

Van Orden, G. C. & H. Kloos 2005. The question of phonology and reading. In Snowling & Hulme (eds) 2005, 61–78.

Varzi, A. C. 2002. On logical relativity. Philosophical Issues, 10: 197–219.

Viger, C. D. 2001. Locking on to the language of thought. Philosophical Psychology, 14: 203–215.

von Kriegstein, K., A. Kleinschmidt, P. Sterzer & A.-L. Giraud 2005. Interaction of face and voice areas during speaker recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17: 367–376.

Wachowitz, S. & J.-P. Ewert 1996. A key by which the toad’s visual system gets access to the domain of prey. Physiology & Behavior, 60: 877–887.

Wakefield, J. C. 2003. Fodor on inscrutability. Mind and Language, 18: 524–537.

Warrington, E. K. & T. Shallice 1984. Category specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107: 829–854.

Wason, P. C. 1966. Reasoning. In B. M. Foss (ed.) New Horizons in Psychology, vol. 1, 135–151. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

Watanabe, T., J. E. Náñez & Y. Sasaki 2001. Perceptual learning without perception. Nature, 413: 844–848.

Watson, J. B. 1925. Behaviorism. New York: People’s Institute.

Wellman, H. M. & S. A. Gelman 1998. Knowledge acquisition in foundational domains. In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (eds) Handbook of Child Psychology, vol. 2, 5th edition, 523–573. New York: Wiley.

Werker, J. F. & R. C. Tees 1984. Cross-language speech perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7: 49–63.

Whiten, A. 2000. Primate culture and social learning. Cognitive Science, 24: 477–508.

Whiten, A., J. Goodall, W. C. McGrew, T. Nishida, V. Reynolds, Y. Sugiyama, C. E. G. Tutin, R. W. Wrangham & C. Boesch 1999. Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399: 682–685.

Wierzbicka, A. 1972. Semantic Primitives (translated by A. Wierzbicka & J. Besemeres). Frankfurt: Athenäum.

Williams, P., I. Gauthier & M. J. Tarr 1998. Feature learning during the acquisition of perceptual expertise. (Commentary on Schyns et al. 1998.) Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21: 40–41.

Wilson, D. 2000. Metarepresentation in linguistic communication. In Sperber (ed.) 2000, 411–448.

Wolfe, J. M. 2003. Moving towards solutions to some enduring controversies in visual search. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7: 70–76.

Woodfield, A. 1986. Two categories of content. Mind and Language, 1: 319–354.

Young, A. W., D. Hellawell & D. C. Hay 1987. Configurational information in face perception. Perception, 16: 747–759.

Metadata

Repository Staff Only: item control page