SCHEMAS  IN  THE  COGNITIVE  AND  CLINICAL  SCIENCES:

 

 

AN  INTEGRATIVE  CONSTRUCT

 

 

 

 

Dan  J.  Stein

 

 

 

 

Dept of Psychiatry,

College of Physicians and Surgeons and the

New York State Psychiatric Institute,

722 W 168 St,

NY, NY 10032

 

 

 

 

        Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to Dan J.

        Stein, M.B., Psychiatric Institute, 722 W 168 St, NY, NY,

        10032.

 

 

 

 

        Key Words:  Schemas, Cognitive science, Psychotherapy

        integration

 

 

 

        Running Head:  Schemas and Integration

 

 

 

        Acknowledgements:  Several anonymous reviewers deserve thanks

        for their extremely helpful comments on earlier drafts of this

        manuscript.

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 ABSTRACT

 

               This paper is concerned in general with the

               intersection of cognitive and clinical science and

               in particular with schema theory.  The use of

               schema theory in the various subdisciplines of

               cognitive science, as well as by cognitive-

               behavioral clinicians and psychoanalytically

               oriented clinicians is reviewed.  It is argued

               that schema theory, in both cognitive and clinical

               sciences, allows a focus on mental structures,

               their biological basis, their development and

               change, and on the way in which they direct

               psychological events.  Schema theory not only

               enables important advances in different clinical

               schools, but it allows central clinical themes to

               be tackled in convergent ways.  It is concluded

               that the schema construct allows integration

               within cognitive science, within the clinic, and

               between the two.

 

 

 

 

 

  

 INTRODUCTION

 

               Psychology has over the last few decades

 

               witnessed a cognitive revolution, and the

 

               multidisciplinary arena known as cognitive science

 

               has increasingly come to the fore as a possible

 

               unifying paradigm for the various psychological

 

               sciences and schools.  Workers within philosophy,

 

               neuroscience, developmental psychology, cognitive

 

               psychology, and social psychology have begun to

 

               use similar constructs (Gardner, 1985).

 

                   Within the clinic, however, there remains

 

               reluctance to accept a unified model of the mind.

 

               Diverse theories and practices abound.

 

               Nevertheless, some have argued that rigorous

 

               research techniques and sophisticated theoretical

 

               ideas have combined to foster a climate for a

 

               unified, eclectic theory and practice of

 

               psychotherapy (Beitman, Goldfried and Norcross,

 

               1989).

 

                   There are some constructs, moreover, that are

 

               being employed not only in the subdisciplines of

 

               cognitive science but also in the clinic, and

 

               indeed cognitive science appears to constitute a

 

               paradigm to which diverse clinicians are attracted

                                     

               (Bowers and Meichenbaum, 1984; Colby and Stoller,

 

               1988; Ingram, 1986; Horowitz, 1988; Ruesch and

 

               Bateson, 1968; Williams, Fraser, MacLeod, and

 

               Matthews, 1988).  Perhaps the most widely known of

 

               the constructs used within both cognitive and

 

               clinical science is that of schemas.  It has been

 

               argued that schemas are a heuristic and useful

 

               concept in cognitive theory (Fiske and Linville,

 

               1980).  In this paper the suggestion is made that

 

               schemas may be a heuristic and integrative notion

 

               in clinical science.  The argument procedes by

 

               noting how cognitive scientists have used schema

 

               theory to focus on the structures of the mind,

 

               their biological basis, their development and

 

               change, and the way in which they direct

 

               psychological events.  The paper then demonstrates

 

               that schemas have been used by clinical scientists

 

               to focus on analogous issues.  It is concluded

 

               that schemas constitute an important construct

 

               that fosters integration within cognitive science,

 

               within the clinic, and between the two.

 

            COGNITIVE SCIENCE AND SCHEMA THEORY

 

               The paper begins with a review of the use of

 

               the schema concept in the various subdisciplines

                                      

 

 

              of cognitive science.  An historical perspective

 

               is employed; the review begins with authors who

 

               were important in laying the foundations for

 

               cognitive science, and moves to a consideration of

 

               contemporary workers.

 

                   a) Philosophy

 

                   Cognitive science may be viewed as an

 

               enterprise that is concerned with the ways in

 

               which the structures of the mind allow

 

               representations of the world, and the ways in

 

               which they process such representations.  The

 

               question of the mind's representation of the world

 

               has long been posed by philosophy, and philosophy

 

               may therefore be considered the founding

 

               subdiscipline of cognitive science (Gardner,

 

               1985).

 

                   One of the greatest philosophers, Kant,

 

               employed the concept of the schema precisely in

 

               order to discuss the possibility of knowledge.

 

               Kant attempted to go beyond the impasse between

 

               the empiricists, who argued that knowledge has its

 

               origins in the external world, and the

 

               rationalists, who argued that knowledge is a

 

               product of the mind.  He argued that schemas

                                      

 

 

               interdigitate between properties of the mind (the

 

               a priori categories) and raw sensory data (of a

 

               posteriori experience).  "This representation of a

 

               universal procedure of the imagination in

 

               providing an image for a concept, I entitle the

 

               schema of the concept" (quoted in Gardner, 1985).

 

               In more contemporary terms, mental schemas are

 

               activated by the external world, and

 

               simultaneously provide an interpretation of it.

 

                b) Neuroscience

 

                Cognitive science acknowledges that structures

 

               of the mind have a biological basis, and

 

               neuroscience is therefore an important

 

               subdiscipline of cognitive science.  Furthermore,

 

               the neurologists Head and Holmes (1911) were among

 

               the first to use the concept of the schema.

 

               These workers were interested in the spatial

 

               perceptions of patients of their bodies, and

 

               referred to the basis of these as the postural

 

               schema.  The postural schema integrated sensations

 

               which were triggered by postural change.  In

 

               lesions of the parietal lobe the schema may be

 

               destroyed, with the possible outcome that patients

 

               ignore part of their body, treating it as if it

                                      

 

 

              were not their own.  Conversely, an amputee may

 

               have an intact brain schema, and therefore

 

               experience movements in the missing phantom limb.

 

               Today the more widely used term is the body schema

 

               (Frederiks, 1969), and contemporary neuroscience

 

               has advanced to the point where it can begin to

 

               consider the biological underpinnings of more

 

               complex schemas such as cognitive and affective

 

               schemas (LeDoux, 1989).

 

               c) Developmental Psychology

 

               Although developmental psychology is not

 

               usually considered one of the subdisciplines of

 

               cognitive science, Piaget is one of the most

 

               important figures in the prehistory of cognitive

 

               science.  The notion of the schema was central in

 

               Piaget's work.

 

               According to Piaget, the initial schemas of

 

               the child comprise biologically based sensorimotor

 

               reflexes which coordinate the child's interactions

 

               with the environment.  Gradually these biological

 

               schemas allow adaptation to the environment by two

 

               complementary processes.  Via assimilation the

 

               schema grasps some novel aspect of the

 

               environnment, so modifying itself to cope with

                                      

 

 

              the environment better.  Via accomodation the

 

               schema is differentiated and elaborated so as to

 

               be consistent with the environment.  With time,

 

               the schemas are transformed to the point where the

 

               organism reaches a new stage of development.

 

               Piaget succeeds in providing a detailed

 

               description of these transformations from the

 

               point of sensorimotor schemas to the operations of

 

               formal thought (Piaget, 1952).

 

              d) Cognitive Psychology

 

               The concept of the schema in contemporary

 

               cognitive science is perhaps most directly

 

               traceable to the work of the British cognitive

 

               psychologist Bartlett (1932).  Bartlett, a onetime

 

               student of Head, was interested in memory, and in

 

               particular in the notion that the context of an

 

               experience had crucial effects on what was

 

               retained and how well this was recalled.

 

               Ebbinghaus had pioneered the experimental study of

 

               memory using nonsense syllables, but this approach

 

               did not seem adequate to Bartlett's concerns.  A

 

               conversation with Norbert Weiner, one of the

 

               founding figures in cognitive science, gave

 

               Bartlett an experimental methodology for

                                       

 

 

              developing his ideas.  Weiner's idea was to use

 

               the Russian Scandal parlor game in which a story

 

               is passed around the room, and then the original

 

               and final versions compared.  Bartlett found that

 

               subjects showed consistent patterns of error in

 

               the recall of narratives.  Thus, for example, an

 

               American Indian narrative would regularly be

 

               revised by subjects until it came to resemble a

 

               Western tale.  Bartlett developed the construct of

 

               the schema to explain this, describing a schema as

 

               a component of memory which is formed from

 

               encounters with the environment, and which

 

               organizes information in specific ways.  Such

 

               schemas aid the recall of a typical (Western)

 

               narrative, but systematically distort the recall

 

               of an unusual (American Indian) narrative.

 

                   Bartlett (1932) wrote,

 

                    "Remembering is not the re-excitation of

 

                    innumerable fixed, lifeless, and

 

                    fragmentary traces.  It is an

 

                    imaginative reconstruction, or

 

                    construction, built out of the relation

 

                    of our attitude towards a whole active

 

                    mass of past experience....It is thus

                                      

 

 

                   hardly ever really exact, even in the

 

                    most rudimentary cases of rote

 

                    recapitulation, and it is not at all

 

                    important that it should be so.  The

 

                    attitude is literally an effect of the

 

                    organism's capacity to turn round up

 

                    upon its own "schemata" and is directly

 

                    a function of consciousness."

 

                   He defined a schema as

 

                    "an active organization of past

 

                    reactions or of past experiences which

 

                    must always be supposed to be operating

 

                    in any well-adapted organic repsonse.

 

                    Whenever there is any order or

 

                    regularity of behavior, a particular

 

                    response is possible only because it is

 

                    related to other similar responses which

 

                    have been serially organized, yet which

 

                    operate not singly as individual members

 

                    coming one after another, but as a

 

                    unitary mass."

 

                   A variety of definitions of schemas have been

 

               offered subsequently.  Thorndyke and Hayes-Roth

 

               (1979) describe three universal assumptions made

                                      

 

 

              by different authors: that a schema is an

 

               organization of conceptually related elements

 

               representing a prototypical abstraction of a

 

               complex concept; that a schema gradually develops

 

               from past experience; and that a schema guides the

 

               organization of new information.  A schema

 

               comprises an architectural element (its structure)

 

               and a propositional element (its content).

 

               Cognitive schemas are involved in cognitive

 

               operations (e.g.  encoding, retrieval), in which

 

               cognitive events (e.g.  thoughts, images) are

 

               produced and processed.  Schemas are highly

 

               interdependent and hierarchically organised, they

 

               may involve verbal or nonverbal-elements, and they

 

               may be more or less open to awareness (Craik and

 

               Lockhart, 1972; Ingram and Kendall, 1986).

 

                   Schema theory has proven valuable in

 

               accounting for a variety of psychological

 

               phenomena.  Schema theory has been used in

 

               contemporary studies of memory (Schacter, 1989),

 

               concept representation (Smith, 1989), problem

 

               solving (VanLehn, 1989), movement (Jordan and

 

               Rosenbaum, 1989) and language (Arbib et al, 1987).

 

               Schemas have been found to facilitate recognition

                                      

 

 

              and recall, to influence speed of information

 

               processing and problem solving and allow for the

 

               chunking of information into more meaningful units

 

               to enable inference about missing data, and to

 

               provide a basis for prediction and decision

 

               making.  It is thought that schemas engender bias

 

               by relying on confirmatory evidence at the expense

 

               of disconfirming evidence, but that this process

 

               maintains schemas (Rumelhart, 1984; Meichenbaum

 

               and Gilmore, 1984; Winfrey and Goldfried, 1986).

 

                   A variety of other constructs have been used

 

               by cognitive scientists and their forerunners to

 

               describe mental structures.  Many bear a good deal

 

               of family resemblance to the idea of schemas.

 

               Notable examples include Abelson and Shank's

 

               (1981) "scripts", Bandura's (1978) "self-systems",

 

               Kelly's (1955) "personal constructs", Miller,

 

               Galanter and Pribram's (1960) "plans", and

 

               Minsky's (1975) "frames".  The notion of the

 

               connectionist network, currently extremely popular

 

               in the field of artifical intelligence, has been

 

               argued to represent a microlevel description of

 

               the schema concept (Ben Zeev, 1988).

 

 

                                      

 

 

              BETWEEN COGNITIVE AND CLINICAL SCIENCE

 

               This review of the schema construct in

 

               cognitive science demonstrates that schema theory

 

               has been widely employed by cognitive scientists.

 

               However, it may be objected that the very

 

               diversity of the use of schema theory points to

 

               the problematic nature of the schema concept.

 

               Different cognitive scientists operationalize and

 

               measure schemas in different ways.  Similarly the

 

               versatility of the concept may reflect only a lack

 

               of theoretical rigor.

 

                   On the other hand the concept of schemas has

 

               general heuristic value insofar as it allows

 

               different cognitive scientists to theorize about

 

               mental structures from the perspective of their

 

               particular subdiscipline.  The schema construct

 

               allows different cognitive scientists to begin to

 

               build an integrative framework that addresses such

 

               questions as how the structures of the mind enable

 

               representation, how they are based in biology, how

 

               they develop and change, and how they account for

 

               a variety of psychological phenomena.

 

                   Certainly there is room in schema research for

 

               improvement in both empirical measurement and

                                      

 

 

               theoretical rigor (Fiske and Linville, 1980;

 

               Williams et al, 1988).  Nevertheless, the

 

               development of the schema concept as a broad

 

               heuristic is important insofar as it represents a

 

               move in cognitive science away from a molecular

 

               and bottom-up approach (concentrating on the

 

               elementary units of processing and on the

 

               influence of the details of a task on

 

               performance), and toward a molar and top-down

 

               approach (highlighting the large-scale properties

 

               of processing and the influence of strategies and

 

               contexts on performance).  Similarly schema theory

 

               represents a move away from the laboratory and

 

               artificial Ebbinghausian methodologies to a

 

               concern with the investigation of day-to-day human

 

               activity.

 

                   These shifts in cognitive science are

 

               reflected in increasing interest in using schema

 

               theory to investigate such phenomena as self and

 

               other representation, and the representation of

 

               emotion.

 

                   Schema theory has been applied to concepts of

 

               the self by a number of workers (Markus, 1977;

 

               Rogers, 1981).  This research follows the line of

                                       

 

 

              earlier work on schemas.  Thus, self schemas are

 

               viewed as generalizations about the self that

 

               develop out of past experience and that organize

 

               the processing of self-related information in the

 

               social environment.  This in turn results in

 

               various forms of bias, but allows the maintenance

 

               of a consistent self-concept (Greenwald, 1980).

 

                   Similarly a number of authors (Cantor and

 

               Mischel, 1977; Fiske, 1981; Taylor and Crocker,

 

               1982) have described evaluative schemas for

 

               assessing others.  In this kind of research,

 

               schemas have been used as to explain such

 

               phenomena as stereotypes and the reactions they

 

               elicit.

 

               Finally, schema theory has begun to grapple

 

               with the problem of emotion.  Early authors

 

               suggested that affect and cognition are seperate

 

               but interactive systems.  Affect was argued to be

 

               either postcognitive (Neisser, 1976; Mandler,

 

               1975) or precognitive (Zajonc, 1980).  On the

 

               other hand, Leventhal (1982) and Greenberg and

 

               Safran (1984) have offered schema models which

 

               attempt to synthesize affect and cognition.

 

               Leventhal (1982), for example, writes that

                                      

 

 

               emotions "....can be regarded as a form of

 

               meaning.  They have significance for the person

 

               experiencing and expressing them.  Their meaning

 

               has two aspects:  they 'say' something about our

 

               organismic state...and they 'say' something about

 

               the environment".  In this line of thinking,

 

               schemas have both an ideational and an affective

 

               component.

 

               All these areas require further empirical and

 

               theoretical development (Higgins and Bargh, 1987;

 

               Klein and Kihlstrom, 1986).  As cognitive

 

               scientists begin to research these subjects, their

 

               interests begin to approximate those of

 

               clinicians.  Let us move, then, to clinical theory

 

               and practice.

 

            CLINICAL SCIENCE AND SCHEMA THEORY

 

                It has been demonstrated that cognitive

 

               scientists use schema theory to address a number

 

               of salient issues about mental structures.  First,

 

               structures of the mind enable representation.

 

               Second, these structures are based in biology.

 

               Third, structures develop and change.  Fourth,

 

               such structures may account for a variety of

 

               psychological phenomena, including behavior,

                                      

 

 

                cognition, and emotion.

 

                With this range of issues in mind, it is not

 

               difficult to see how schema theory may be useful

 

               in the clinic.  Psychopathology may involve, for

 

               example, conflict (where underlying schemas are

 

               somehow distorted) or deficit (where underlying

 

               schemas are absent or underdeveloped).

 

               Psychopathologists are interested, then, in

 

               disturbed representations, in particular in

 

               disturbances in representations of self, other,

 

               and the interaction of self and other.  Insofar as

 

               these disturbances have a biological underpinning,

 

               this too is of interest.  The developmental

 

               transformations which lead to such disturbances

 

               are of course of great importance.  Models of

 

               psychopathology attempt to describe the psychic

 

               structures that lead to abnormal behavior,

 

               cognition, and emotion.

 

               Similarly, psychotherapy may involve, for

 

               example, transference (where the therapist is seen

 

               through pre-existing schemas), resistance (where

 

               pre-existing schemas undermine change) and

 

               interpretation (where a meta-cognitive process of

 

               disengagement from and reflection on the patient's

                                     

 

 

               schemas occurs).  Psychotherapists are interested,

 

               then, in changes in representations of self and

 

               other, and in the way in which psychotherapy leads

 

               to such changes.  Insofar as psychopharmacological

 

               interventions help promote such changes in

 

               representations, therapists will be interested in

 

               how this occurs.  Models of psychotherapy attempt

 

               to describe the psychic structures that account

 

               for various phenomena in psychotherapy that

 

               involve behavior, cognition, and emotion.

 

               While these concerns are common to different

 

               psychopathologists and psychotherapists, many

 

               concepts other than that of schemas are, of

 

               course, employed to explain psychopathology and

 

               psychotherapy.  I want to show that although such

 

               concepts may be useful, they may also be open to

 

               transformation into, or subsumption under, schema

 

               theory.  It may be objected that this merely

 

               constitutes the translation of different terms

 

               into schema language.  However, I want to argue

 

               that schema theory allows different clinical

 

               schools to include rigorous components of their

 

               orientation and to discard less rigorous aspects,

 

               and so enables advances in these schools.

                                      

 

 

               Furthermore, schema theory fosters integration by

 

               allowing central themes to be tackled in

 

               convergent ways.

 

                These arguments are made in the following

 

               sections in terms of two divergent approaches in

 

               modern clinical work, the cognitive-behavioral and

 

               the psychoanalytic.  Schema theory may be seen as

 

               increasingly important in each school.  It will be

 

               suggested that schema theory enables important

 

               advances within each school, and that it allows

 

               important clinical themes to be considered in an

 

               integrative way.

 

             COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL THERAPY AND SCHEMAS

 

               The initial behavioral model of

 

               psychopathology and psychotherapy was that of

 

               Watson.  The exemplar discussed by Watson and his

 

               students (Watson and Rayner, 1920) was the

 

               induction and elimination of a fear of white rats

 

               in an eleven month old boy, Albert B., with

 

               techniques of conditioning and deconditioning.  A

 

               critique of this discussion is beyond the scope of

 

               this paper, but it may be noted that such work

 

               does not readily allow a focus on some of the

 

               concerns that psychopathologists and

                                     

 

 

              psychotherapists have been listed as having.

 

               Mental structures are pointedly ignored,

 

               constitutional factors are downplayed,

 

               developmental processes are limited to those of

 

               conditioning, and while behavioral events such as

 

               phobias and learning are tackled, other complex

 

               phenomena in psychopathology and psychotherapy

 

               such as personality disorder, or resistance and

 

               transferance, are ignored.  It is not surprising,

 

               then, that there has been a shift in clinical

 

               behaviorism to more cognitive models.

 

                While the early cognitive-behaviorists, such

 

               as Ellis (1962), incorporated cognitions into

 

               their theory, there was still a reluctance to

 

               focus on the structures of the mind.  Abnormal

 

               cognitions resulted from cognitive distortions,

 

               and the structures that produced cognitive

 

               distortions were glossed over.  There was a

 

               corresponding neglect of the development of

 

               cognitive distortions.  It may be again be argued

 

               that this view has difficulty in accounting for

 

               complex kinds of psychopathology and for complex

 

               phenomena in psychotherapy.

 

                   However, later cognitive-behaviorists have

                                      

 

 

               become increasingly interested in structures of

 

               the mind.  Beck, for example, introduced the

 

               schema concept in the context of depression (Beck,

 

               1964, 1967), and more recently he and his

 

               colleagues (1990) have elaborated the notion of

 

               schemas in order to extend cognitive therapy to

 

               the personality disorders.  Other authors in the

 

               cognitive-behavioral tradition have also employed

 

               the notion of schemas (Arnkoff, 1980; Turk and

 

               Speer, 1983; Goldfried and Robins, 1983; Greenberg

 

               and Safran, 1984; Young, 1990), but Beck's is the

 

               most extensive contribution.

 

                   Beck and his co-workers suggest that different

 

               types of schemas have different functions.  In

 

               their view cognitive schemas are concerned with

 

               abstraction, interpretation and recall, affective

 

               schemas are responsible for the generation of

 

               feelings, motivational schemas deal with wishes

 

               and desires, instrumental schemas prepare for

 

               action, and control schemas are involved with

 

               self-monitoring and self-regulation.  There are

 

               also subsystems of schemas.  Thus cognitive

 

               schemas include schemas concerned with

 

               self-evaluation and evaluation of others, schemas

                                      

 

 

              concerned with memory and recall, and schemas

 

               concerned with expectancies and predictions.  They

 

               argue that such schemas can be viewed as operating

 

               in logical linear progression.  A dangerous

 

               stimulus activates a "danger schema", which in

 

               turn activates affective , motivational, action,

 

               and control schemas.

 

                   This framework is used to discuss development

 

               and change.  Schemas develop as a result of both

 

               constitutional and environmental factors.  A

 

               particular schema may develop in response to

 

               developmental experiences, or as a result of

 

               identification with family members.  Certain

 

               patterns may be derived from or compensate for

 

               such schemas.  Therapists use cognitive, affective

 

               and behavioral strategies to change cognitive,

 

               affective, and behavioral schemas.  Change

 

               comprises a continuum from restructuring of

 

               schemas to more subtle modifications.

 

                   This framework is also used to discuss

 

               different kinds of psychopathology.  Dysfunctional

 

               thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are due to

 

               schemas that produce consistently biased

 

               judgements.  The schemas of personality disorders

                                      

 

 

              resemble those of the symptom disorders, but they

 

               are involved in the everyday processing of

 

               information, and are operative on a more

 

               continuous basis. 

 

                   Finally this model is used to discuss complex

 

               aspects of psychotherapy.  Schemas regarding

 

               change, self, and others may impede therapy.

 

               Schemas regarding the therapist may be labelled

 

               transferance cognitions.

 

              PSYCHOANALYTIC THERAPY AND SCHEMAS

 

               Classical psychoanalytic theory posits that

 

               psychopathology can be understood in terms of an

 

               energetics model.  Freud argued that the forces of

 

               the unconscious are expressed, repressed, and

 

               transformed, resulting in everyday behaviors and

 

               in psychopathology.  During development these

 

               forces manifest in different configurations, and

 

               developmental events may therefore affect later

 

               dynamics in specific ways.  Psychoanalysis leads

 

               to insight into such dynamics, and allows for

 

               their working through.

 

                   Modern analysts have increasingly rejected

 

               this model, which is drawn from nineteenth century

 

               science.  Many have attempted to de-emphasize the

                                      

 

 

              drive component of psychoanalytic theory, and to

 

               focus on its relational aspects; on the self, the

 

               object, and their interaction (Mitchell, 1988).

 

               On this model, the past is important insofar as

 

               previous relationships determine the nature of

 

               future ones.  Symptoms are explicable, for

 

               example, in terms of relational deficits.  Therapy

 

               occurs within the context of an empathic

 

               relationship, and so allows for growth.

 

                   Furthermore, in considering new models to

 

               replace the energetic one, a number of

 

               psychoanalysts have drawn on schema theory (Eagle,

 

               1986; Slap and Saykin 1983; Wachtel, 1982).

 

               Perhaps the most extensive contribution, however,

 

               is that of Horowitz (1988, in press).

 

                   Horowitz (1988) suggests that every individual

 

               has a repertoire of multiple schemas of self and

 

               other.  Self schemas include ways to gain pleasure

 

               or avoid displeasure (motivational schemas),

 

               positioning in relation to the world (role

 

               schemas), and ways of helping a person decide

 

               which of two motives to choose (value schemas).

 

               Superordinate schemas that articulate self to

 

               other schemas in ways that organize a script of

                                     

 

 

              wishes, fears, and likely reactions are called

 

               role relationship models.  Schemas are applied

 

               simultaneously along multiple parallel channels.

 

               Shifts in schema activity are accompanied by

 

               shifts in state of mind, and control processes

 

               prevent the emergence of certain states of mind in

 

               defensive fashion.

 

                   This model is again used to account for

 

               development and change.  Self-schemas develop from

 

               a basis of genetic and environmental interaction.

 

               Role relationship models develop in the context of

 

               interpersonal behavioral patterns.  Common issues

 

               are those of love and care, power and control, and

 

               sexuality and status.

 

                   The framework is also used to discuss

 

               different kinds of psychopathology.  Changes in

 

               the environment may lead to a poor fit between

 

               external reality and preexisting role-relationship

 

               models.  Emotional response to stress is due in

 

               part to the discord between the new situation and

 

               enduring schemas.  In some people schemas and

 

               patterns of control of schemas are inappropriate

 

               and insistent.  These constitute personality

 

               problems.

                                      

 

 

                  Finally this model is used to account for the

 

               phenomena of psychotherapy.  In therapy the

 

               schemas of the patient become manifest in the

 

               role-relationship models between clinician and

 

               patient.  The therapeutic alliance allows the

 

               patient to develop insight into such schemas, and

 

               during a process of working through to gradually

 

               replace or modify them.

 

            AN INTEGRATIVE APPROACH

 

                   While schema theory may be increasingly

 

               important in both cognitive-behavioral and

 

               psychodynamic work, Beck and Horowitz approach

 

               clinical theory and practice from widely divergent

 

               positions.  This is reflected in their assumptions

 

               about schemas.  Beck's view of schemas as

 

               triggered in a linear fashion is consistent with

 

               the focus of cognitive therapy on the automatic

 

               cognitions that precede emotion.  Horowitz's

 

               division of schemas into motivational, role and

 

               value schemas, on the other hand, reflects the

 

               Freudian concern with the id, ego, and superego

 

               structures.  Furthermore, differences in

 

               theoretical assumptions lead to divergent methods

 

               for measuring schemas.

                                      

 

 

                  Nevertheless, as in the case of schema theory

 

               in cognitive science, the schema concept may have

 

               a broad heuristic value in the clinic insofar as

 

               it allows different clinicians to approach common

 

               concerns about mental structures, their

 

               development, and their importance in understanding

 

               psychopathology and psychotherapy.  The work of

 

               Beck and Horowitz on schemas converges on these

 

               subjects.

 

                   This section attempts to illustrate the

 

               heuristic value of schema theory in the clinic.

 

               It is argued first that the cognitive-behaviorist

 

               and psychoanalytic models each have distinct

 

               advantages and disadvantages, and that schema

 

               theory has potential for incorporating the

 

               advantages and eschewing the disadvantages of

 

               these schools.  It is then suggested that schema

 

               theory allows a number of important clinical

 

               themes to be considered in an integrative way.

 

                   One advantage of behaviorism lies in is its

 

               emphasis on a systematic and empirical approach.

 

               The model presented here is consistent with the

 

               idea that it is possible to study psychopathology

 

               and psychotherapy in a systematic way, and to

                                      

 

 

              provide empirical descriptions of psychological

 

               phenomena.  Clinicians have begun to devote

 

               attention to the rigorous assessment of schemas

 

               (Landau and Goldfried, 1981; Rudy and Merluzzi,

 

               1984; Segal, 1988), and such methodologies are

 

               important in research on the nature of

 

               psychopathology and on the efficacy of

 

               psychotherapy.  Furthermore, while it may be

 

               argued that the advance of clinical theory will

 

               always be intimately tied to clinical practice,

 

               schema theory allows for the incorporation of more

 

               general experimental techniques and findings.

 

                   On the other hand, the cognitive-behavioral

 

               tradition has increasingly chosen to theorize

 

               about the mental structures that early

 

               behaviorists wanted to ignore.  Early cognitive-

 

               behaviorists noted abnormal cognitions, but

 

               continued to de-emphasize the structures

 

               responsible for these cognitions, their

 

               development, and their manifestation in the clinic

 

               in the form of resistance and transferance.

 

               Finally, modern cognitive- behavioral therapists

 

               such as Guidano and Liotti (1983) have devoted a

 

               good deal of attention to psychological

                                     

 

 

              structures, to their development, and to their

 

               importance in psychopathology and psychotherapy.

 

               It is at this point that cognitive-behavioral

 

               theory begins to employ a schema model.

 

                   In contrast to behaviorism, psychoanalysis

 

               tackles mental structures without hesitation.  In

 

               many ways, Freud may be considered a cognitivist

 

               who focused on mental structures, their growth and

 

               change, and their consequences for behavior and

 

               for the clinical situation.

 

                   On the other hand, Freud never really escaped

 

               his natural science background.  The theory of

 

               drive permeates all his works.  While the drive

 

               model constitutes a good model for such phenomena

 

               as movement and momentum, it fails to provide an

 

               adequate account of the subtle meanderings and

 

               manipulations of the mind.  Drive and force are

 

               too cumbersome for meaning and cognition.

 

               Further, Freud's notion of drives is one which

 

               conceives of the child's unconscious as identical

 

               to that of the adult, and his notion of memory

 

               traces is in terms of a passive perceptual

 

               apparatus; his usual penetrating focus on

 

               development and transformation blurrs at this

                                      

 

 

               crucial point.  Finally, the inherently conflicted

 

               notion of an energy that is psychic exemplifies

 

               Freud's failure to resolve the relation between

 

               psyche and soma.  Modern analysts have retained

 

               aspects of drive theory, but have shifted their

 

               emphasis to the construction of the self, of the

 

               other, and of the space between them (Mitchell,

 

               1988).  This view rejects drives (as energetic or

 

               psychological concepts), without going to the

 

               opposite extreme of denying that psychoanalysis is

 

               a science concerned with models.  It is at this

 

               point that psychodynamic theory begins to adopt a

 

               schema model.

 

                   Given the kinds of approximations outlined

 

               here, it is not surprising to find an overlap of

 

               various techniques in clinical practice.  Modern

 

               cognitive therapists work with dreams and

 

               spontaneous images (Young, 1990), while

 

               psychoanalysts have become increasingly open to

 

               the adoption of cognitive-behavioral techniques

 

               (Wachtel, 1977).

 

                   Further, the possibility of increasing

 

               theoretical integration emerges.  By way of

 

               example let us use schema theory to consider some

                                      

 

 

               central focuses of psychotherapy - the

 

               unconscious, drives and defenses, symptoms and

 

               therapeutic change.

 

                   Insofar as schemas are not known to the

 

               person, they are unconscious.  The importance of

 

               unconscious processes has become increasingly

 

               apparent in cognitive science (Kihlstrom, 1988).

 

               It is clear that the processes that take place out

 

               of awareness are not only extremely important, but

 

               can be scientifically described.  While empirical

 

               clinical research on the relation between schemas

 

               and the unconscious is limited (Shevrin, 1988),

 

               schema theory may be theoretically useful in the

 

               clinic.  Consider, for example, a female victim of

 

               incest, who describes difficulties in

 

               relationships with men, but who is largely unaware

 

               of how these relationships reflect earlier

 

               familial patterns.  It is possible to

 

               conceptualize this patient as having particular

 

               schemas about herself, about men, and about her

 

               interactions with men; schemas of which she may be

 

               or less aware.  Thus this patient may have schemas

 

               in which atttention is given her by men only when

 

               she is seductive, and in which all men are

                                      

 

 

              untrustworthy.  The patient may consciously deny

 

               the presence of such schemas but nevertheless her

 

               thoughts, feelings and actions may entail their

 

               existence.  Thus a clinical formulation is

 

               constructed which posits particular unconscious

 

               schemas, and which is employed to account for

 

               consciously reported psychological phenomena.

 

               This schema view of the unconscious discards the

 

               classical analytic metaphor of the unfettered

 

               beast lurking in the mind, as well as the modern

 

               analytic metaphor of the damaged baby at the core

 

               of the patient.  It also departs from the

 

               behaviorist avoidance of the unconscious.  The

 

               schema view, however, maintains the classic

 

               analytic ideas of psychic determinism and of the

 

               existence of unconscious processes, and also

 

               conforms to the attempts of modern analysts and

 

               behavioral-cognitive therapists to describe how

 

               the past repeats itself unwittingly in the

 

               present.

 

                   Insofar as schemas direct behaviors,

 

               cognitions, and emotions in a particular way, they

 

               result in repetitive patterns of behavior,

 

               thought, and feeling.  However, schemas develop in

                                       

 

 

              adaptation to life, and can therefore be seen not

 

               only as constrictive (defensive) but also as

 

               creative (strategic).  Thus in the patient

 

               discussed earlier the schema in which all men are

 

               viewed as untrustworthy, may have proved helpful

 

               at home, but may have lead to inappropriate affect

 

               in later relationships.  Analysts may object to

 

               this kind of thinking for it appears to downplay

 

               the libidinal and aggressive phenomena which are

 

               highlighted in analytic defense theory.  Certainly

 

               schema theory has only begun to theorize affect,

 

               sexuality and aggression (Horowitz, 1988).

 

               Nevertheless, the idea that schemas have defensive

 

               and strategic aspects discards the classical

 

               notion of energetic forces being defended against

 

               by the ego, and downplays the modern analytic

 

               emphasis on psychic deficit as accounting for the

 

               repetitive patterns of the present.  However, this

 

               view can perhaps incorporate the classic analytic

 

               concern with affect, as well as the modern

 

               analytic and cognitive-behavioral emphasis on the

 

               adaptive or strategic aspects of fixed patterns of

 

               behavior, thought, and feeling.

 

                   Insofar as the behaviors, cognitions, and

                                      

 

 

               emotions directed by schemas are abnormal, they

 

               constitute symptoms.  Psychotic symptoms may, for

 

               example, point to the absence or lack of

 

               development of certain symptoms (Perris, 1990).

 

               Character symptoms may indicate the presence of

 

               maladaptive schemas (Young, 1990).  Dissociative

 

               symptoms may point to a lack of integration

 

               between schemas (Hilgard, 1977).  Schemas in the

 

               patient mentioned earlier may, for example, result

 

               in personality features such as being overly

 

               seductive, or being quick to anger.  Further work

 

               needs to be done on the way in which schema theory

 

               acccounts for the range of clinical symptoms.  It

 

               is clear, however, that a schema theory of

 

               symptoms contrasts with the classical analytic

 

               notion of symptoms as dynamic compromise

 

               formations, the modern analytic emphasis on

 

               symptoms as the result of relational deficits, the

 

               behaviorist concept of symptoms as

 

               stimulus-response mediated, and the early

 

               cognitivist description of symptoms as emerging

 

               from cognitive distortions.  Schema theory does,

 

               however, follow psychoanalytic and

 

               cognitive-behavioral theory insofar as they

                                      

 

 

              emphasize the mental structures responsible for

 

               symptoms.

 

                   What about therapeutic change?  According to

 

               schema theory, therapy involves an affective and

 

               cognitive process in which a variety of techniques

 

               are used to engage and explore schemas.

 

               Subsequent events depend on the nature of the

 

               pre-existing schemas.  Some schemas may be

 

               non-linguistic in nature, and these may then be

 

               given a voice.  Other schemas may be out of

 

               awareness, and these may then be acknowledged.

 

               The patient may be encouraged not to use a

 

               particular schema or to develop compensatory

 

               schemas (Ingram and Hollon, 1986).  Alternatively

 

               schema change can be fostered, either of more

 

               superficial schemas or of deeper ones, using

 

               emotive, cognitive and behavioral methods (Young,

 

               1990).  In the patient we have discussed, the

 

               operation of schemas of seduction and mistrust

 

               within the therapeutic relationship, and the way

 

               in which they lead to particular thoughts and

 

               feelings may be brought into awareness, and the

 

               patient may be encouraged to practice new ways of

 

               relating to others.  This kind of theorizing

                                       

 

 

              contrasts with the classic analytic model of cure

 

               as the result of dynamic catharsis, the modern

 

               analytic model of cure as the result of empathic

 

               reparenting, and the cognitive-behavioral models

 

               of reinforcement or relearning.  Schema theory

 

               nevertheless maintains the Freudian emphasis on

 

               insight, the modern analytic emphasis on empathy,

 

               and the cognitive notion of the remodelling of

 

               mental structures.  In similar vein, transferance

 

               involves not so much a distorted neurotic

 

               repetition, nor a genuine holding environment, but

 

               rather an entry into and reshaping of the patterns

 

               of the patient.

 

                   The cumulative effect of these sketches of the

 

               clinical application of schemas hopefully

 

               indicates the integrative potential of this

 

               construct.  Nevertheless much work remains to be

 

               done.  Each of these clinical sketches requires

 

               theoretical expansion.  Theoretical work at the

 

               overlap of cognitive and clinical science, such as

 

               the use of schemas to understand self and affect,

 

               also requires further development (Williams et al,

 

               1988).  Furthermore, such theory must be

 

               complemented by empirical research.  If schemas

                                       

 

 

              are to be accepted as real mental structures which

 

               explain mental and clinical phenomena, then their

 

               accurate definition, and documentation become

 

               paramount.  The operationalization and measurement

 

               of deep constructs is, however, difficult (Segal,

 

               1988).  Schemas are measured only indirectly, for

 

               example, by pencil-and-paper methodologies (Coyne

 

               and Gotlib, 1986).  Furthermore, different

 

               cognitive scientists and clinicians have defined

 

               schemas in somewhat different ways, with each

 

               worker attaching his or her theoretical framework

 

               to the construct.  While schemas have a degree of

 

               constancy, they may also be dynamic and

 

               multifaceted, so exacerbating the difficulties of

 

               the empiricist.  In sum, both theoretical

 

               innovation and detailed clinical observation will

 

               be necessary in order to advance an adequate

 

               account of schemas in the clinic.

 

              SUMMARY

 

                   I have shown that schema theory is used by a

 

               variety of cognitive theorists and clinicians.  I

 

               have suggested that schema theory allows a focus

 

               on mental structures, their biological basis,

 

               their development and change, and the way in which

                                       

 

 

              they direct psychological events.  These focuses

 

               are important not only in cognitive science, but

 

               also in the clinic.

 

                   Schema theory therefore allows cognitive

 

               scientists and clinicians to conduct a convergent

 

               discourse.  Furthermore, schema theory may be

 

               useful in allowing different clinicians to employ

 

               a unified framework.

 

                   I have also indicated that a great deal of

 

               work in both the cognitive and clinical sciences

 

               remains to be done.  Problems in the

 

               operationalization and measurement of schemas

 

               remain to be solved, and a variety of theoretical

 

               areas need to be developed further.

 

                   Nevertheless, the use of schema theory in the

 

               clinic opens up cognitive science to a whole

 

               series of phenomena.  The clinic confronts schema

 

               theory with a variety of psychological structures

 

               (cognitive and affective), processes (top-down or

 

               molecular and bottom-up or molar), and forms of

 

               representation (fantasies, dreams, hypnotic

 

               recollections, hallucinations, delusions).  Such a

 

               confrontation benefits cognitive science insofar

 

               as it shifts its focus away from the laboratory

                                     

 

 

                and toward man in all his complexity.

 

                   Conversely, by looking at cognitive science,

 

               clinicians may begin to incorporate empirical and

 

               experimental methodologies.  Cognitive theory may

 

               provide a unified model of the mind to underpin an

 

               unified clinical approach.

 

                   Hopefully this paper will encourage the

 

               dialogue between the cognitive and clinical

 

               sciences, and will encourage empirical and

 

               theoretical research on schemas.

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

                 REFERENCES

 

                   Abelson, R.P.  (1981).  Psychological status

 

               of the script concept.  American Psychologist, 36,

                                      

               715-729.

 

                   Arbib, M.A., Conklin, E.J., & Hill, J.C.

 

               (1987).  From schema theory to language.  Oxford:

                       

               Oxford University Press.

 

                   Arnkoff, A. (1980).  Psychotherapy from the

 

               perspective of cognitive theory.  In M.J.  Mahoney

 

               (Ed.), Psychotherapy process:  Current issues and

                     

               future directions (pp.  339-361).  New York:

              

               Plenum Press.

 

                   Bandura, A. (1978).  The self-system in

 

               reciprocal determinism.  American Psychologist,

                                       

               33, 344-358.

             

                   Bartlett, F. (1932).  Remembering.  Cambridge:

                                       

               Cambridge University Press.

 

                   Beck, A.T.  (1964).  Thinking and depression:

 

               II.  Theory and Therapy.  Archives of General

                                       

               Psychiatry, 10, 561-571.

              

                   Beck, A.T.  (1967).  Depression:  Clinical,

                                      

               experimental and theoretical aspects.  New York:

              

               Harper and Row.

 

                   Beck, A.T., Freeman, A., & Associates (1990):

 

 

 

              Cognitive therapy of personality disorders.  New

              

               York:  Guilford.

 

                   Beitman, B.D., Goldfried, M.R., & Norcross

 

               J.C.  (1989) The movement toward integrating the

 

               psychotherapies:  An overview.  American Journal

                                              

               of Psychiatry, 146, 138-147.

              

                   Ben-Zeev, A. (1988).  The schema paradigm in

 

               perception.  Journal of Mind and Behavior, 9,

                          

               487-514.

 

                   Bowers, K.S., & Meichenbaum, D (Eds.).

 

               (1984).  The unconscious reconsidered.  New York:

                       

               Wiley.

 

                   Cantor, N., Mischel, W. (1977).  Traits as

 

               prototypes:  Effects on recognition memory,

 

               Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35,

              

               38-48.

 

                   Colby, K.M., & Stoller, R.J.  (1988).

 

               Cognitive science and psychoanalysis.  Hillsdale:

              

               Analytic Press.

 

                   Coyne, J.C., & Gotlib, I.H.  (1986).  Studying

 

               the role of cognition in depression.  Well trodden

 

               paths and cul-de-sacs.  Cognitive Therapy and

                                     

               Research, 19, 794-812.

             

                   Craik, F.I.M., & Lockhart, R.S.  (1972).

 

 

 

             Levels of processing:  A framework for memory

 

               research.  Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal

                         

               Behavior, 11, 671-684.

              

                   Eagle, M.N.  (1986).  The psychoanalytic and

 

               the cognitive unconscious.  In R. Stern (Ed.),

 

               Theories of the unconscious (pp.  155-190).

              

               Hillsdale, Analytic Press.

 

                   Ellis, A. (1962).  Reason and emotion in

                                     

               psychotherapy.  Secaucus:  Lyle Stuart and Citadel

               

               Press.

 

                   Fiske, S.T.  (1981).  Schema-triggered affect:

 

               Application to social perception.  In M.S.  Clark

 

               & S.T.  Fiske (Eds.), Affect and cognition (pp.

                                     

               55-78).  Hillsdale:  Erlbaum.

 

                   Fiske, S.T., Linville P.W.  (1980).  What does

 

               the schema concept buy us?  Personality and Social

                                          

               Psychology Bulletin, 6, 543-557.

              

                   Frederiks, J.A.M.  Disorders of the body

 

               schema (1969).  In P.J.  Vinken & G.W.  Bruyn

 

               (Eds.), Handbook of clinical neurology 4.

                      

               Disorders of speech perception and symbolic

 

               behavior (pp.  373-394).  Amsterdam:  Elsevier.

 

                   Gardner, H. (1985).  The mind's new science:

                                       

               A history of the cognitive revolution.  New York:

              

 

 

               Basic Books.

 

                   Goldfried, M.R., Robins C. (1983).

 

               Self-schemata, cognitive bias, and the processing

 

               of therapeutic experiences.  In P.C.  Kendall

 

               (Ed.), Advances in Cognitive-Behavioral Research

                     

               and Therapy, vol 2 (pp.  33-80).  San Diego:

              

               Academic Press.

 

                   Greenberg, L.S., Safran, J.D.  (1984).

 

               Integrating affect and cognition:  A perspective

 

               on the process of therapeutic change.  Cognitive

                                                     

               Therapy and Research, 8, 559-578.

              

                   Greenwald, A.F.  (1980).  The totalitarian

 

               ego:  Fabrication and revision of personal

 

               history.  American Psychologist, 35, 603-618.

                       

                   Guidano, V.F., & Liotti, G. (1983).  Cognitive

                                                     

               processes and emotional disorders:  A structural

              

               approach to psychotherapy.  New York:  Guilford.

              

                   Head, H., Holmes, G. (1911).  Sensory

 

               disturbances from cerebral lesions.  Brain, 34,

                                                

               102-254.

 

                   Higgins, E.T., & Bargh, J. (1987).  Social

 

               cognition and social perception.  Annual Review of

                                                

               Psychology, 38, 369-425.

             

                   Hilgard, E.R.  (1977).  Divided consciousness:

                                          

 

 

             Multiple controls in human thought and action.

              

               New York:  Wiley.

 

                   Horowitz, M.J.  (1988).  Introduction to

                                          

               psychodynamics:  A new synthesis.  New York:

              

               Basic Books.

 

                   Horowitz, M.J.  (in press).  Person Schemas.

 

               In M.J.  Horowitz (Ed.), Person Schemas and

                                      

               Maladaptive Interpersonal Behavior Patterns.

              

               Chicago:  University of Chicago Press.

 

                   Ingram, R.E.  (Ed.).  (1986).  Information

                                                

               processing approaches to clinical psychology.  San

              

               Diego:  Academic Press.

 

                   Ingram, R.E., & Hollon, S.D.  (1986) Cognitive

 

               therapy of depression from an information

 

               processing perspective.  In R.E.  Ingram (Ed.),

 

               Information processing approaches to cognitive

              

               psychology (pp.  261-284).  San Diego:  Academic

             

               Press.

 

                   Ingram, R.E., & Kendall, P.C.  (1986).

 

               Cognitive clinical psychology:  Implications of an

 

               information processing Perspective.  In R.E.

 

               Ingram (Ed.), Information processing approaches to

                            

               clinical psychology (pp.  4-22).  San Diego:

              

               Academic Press.

 

 

 

               Jordan, M.I., Rosenbaum, D.A.  (1989).

 

               Action.  In M.I.  Posner (Ed.), The foundations of

                                             

               cognitive science (pp.  727-768).  Cambridge:  MIT

              

               Press.

 

                   Kelly, G. (1955).  The psychology of personal

                                     

               constructs.  New York:  Norton.

             

                   Kihlstrom, J.F.  (1987).  The cognitive

 

               unconscious, Science, 237, 1145-1151.

                         

                   Klein, S.B., & Kihlstrom, J.F.  (1986).

 

               Elaboration, organization, and the self-reference

 

               effect in memory.  Journal of Experimental

                                 

               Psychology, 115, 26-38.

              

                   Landau, R.J., Goldfried, M.R.  (1981).  The

 

               assessment of semantic structure:  A unifying

 

               focus in cognitive, traditional, and behavioral

 

               assessment.  In P.C.  Kendall & S.D.  Hollon

 

               (Eds.), Assessment stategies for

                     

               cognitive-behavioral interventions.  San Diego:

              

               Academic Press.

 

                   LeDoux J.E.  (1989).  Cognitive-Emotional

 

               interactions in the brain.  Cognition and Emotion,

                                          

               3, 267-289.

              

                   Leventhal, H. (1982).  The integration of

 

               emotion and cognition:  A view from the

 

 

 

               perceptual-motor theory of emotion.  In M.S.

 

               Clarke & S.T.  Fiske (Eds.), Affect and cognition

                                           

               (pp.  121-156).  Hillsdale:  Erlbaum.

 

                   Mandler, G. (1975).  Mind and emotion.  New

                                     

               York:  Wiley.

 

                   Markus, H. (1977).  Self-schemata and

 

               processing information about the self.  Journal of

                                                     

               Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 63-78.

              

                   Meichenbaum, D., & Gilmore, J.B.  (1984).  The

 

               nature of unconscious processes:  A

 

               cognitive-behavioral perspective.  In K.S.  Bowers

 

               & D. Meichenbaum (Eds.), The unconscious

                                        

               reconsidered.  New York:  Wiley.

              

                   Miller, G., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K.

 

               (1960).  Plans and the structure of behavior.  New

                       

               York:  Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

 

                   Minsky, M. (1975).  A framework for

 

               representing knowledge.  In P. Winston (Ed.), The

                                                          

               psychology of computer vision.  New York:

              

               McGraw-Hill.

 

                   Mitchell, S.A.  (1988).  Relational concepts

                                           

               in psychoanalysis:  An integration.  Cambridge:

              

               Harvard University Press.

 

                   Neisser, U. (1976).  Cognition and Reality.

                                       

                      San Francisco:  WH Freeman.

 

               Perris, C. (1990).  Cognitive therapy of

                                      

               schizophrenia.  New York:  Guilford.

             

                   Piaget, J. (1952).  The origins of

                                      

               intelligence in children.  New York:

              

               International Universities Press.

 

                   Rogers, T.B.  (1981).  A model of the self as

 

               an aspect of the human information processing

 

               system.  In N. Cantor & J.F.  Kihlstrom (Eds.),

 

               Personality, cognition, and social interaction

              

               (pp.  193-213).  Hillsdale:  Erlbaum.

 

                   Rudy T.E., & Merluzzi, T.V.  (1984).

 

               Recovering social cognitive schemata:

 

               Descriptions and applications of multidimensional

 

               scaling for clinical research.  In P.C.  Kendall

 

               (Ed.), Advances in cognitive-behavioral research

                    

               and therapy, vol 3 (pp.  61-102).  San Diego:

             

               Academic Press.

 

                   Ruesch, J. & Bateson, G. (1968).

 

               Communication:  The social matrix of psychiatry.

              

               New York:  Norton.

 

                   Rumelhart, D.E.  (1984).  Schemata and the

 

               cognitive system.  In R.S.  Wyer & T.K.  Scrull

 

               (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition.  Hillsdale:

                     

                    Erlbaum.

 

                 

 

            Schacter, D.L.  (1989).  Memory.  In M.I.

 

               Posner (Ed.), The foundations of cognitive science

                           

               (pp.  683-726).  Cambridge:  MIT Press.

 

                   Segal, Z.V.  (1988).  Appraisal of the

 

               self-schemata construct in cognitive models of

 

               depression.  Psychological Bulletin, 103, 147-162.

                         

                   Shevrin, H. (1988).  Unconscious conflict:  A

 

               convergent psychodynamic and electrophysiological

 

               approach.  In M.J.  Horowitz (Ed.), Psychodynamics

                                                 

               and cognition (pp.  169-190).  Chicago:

             

               University of Chicago Press.

 

                   Slap, J.W., & Saykin, A.J.  (1983).  The

 

               schema:  Basic concept in a nonmetapsychological

 

               model of the mind.  Psychoanalysis and

                                  

               Contemporary Thought, 6, 305-325.

               

                   Smith, E.E.  (1989).  Concepts and Induction.

 

               In M.I.  Posner (Ed.), The foundations of

                                     

               cognitive science (pp.  501-526).  Cambridge:  MIT

              

               Press.

 

                   Taylor, S., Crocker, J. (1982).  Schematic

 

               bases of social information processing.  In E.T.

 

               Higgins, C.P.  Herman, & M.P.  Zanna (Eds.),

 

               Social cognition:  The Ontario symposium on

                        personality and social psychology (pp.  81-134).

                            Hillsdale:  Erlbaum.

 

               

 

             Thorndyke, P.W., Hayes-Roth, B. (1979).  The

 

               use of schemata in the acquisition and

 

               transference of knowledge.  Cognitive Psychology,

                                        

               11, 82-106.

             

                   Turk, D.C., & Speers M.A.  (1983).  Cognitive

 

               schemata and cognitive processes in

 

               cognitive-behavioral interventions:  going beyond

 

               the information given.  In P.C.  Kendall (Ed.),

 

               Advances in cognitive-behavioral research and

              

               therapy, vol 2 (pp.  1-31).  San Diego:  Academic

             

               Press.

 

                   VanLehn, K. (1989).  Problem solving and

 

               cognitive skill acquisition.  In M.I.  Posner

 

               (Ed.), The foundations of cognitive science

                     

               (pp.527-580).  Cambridge:  MIT Press.

 

                   Wachtel, P.L.  (1977).  Psychoanalysis and

                                          

               behavior therapy:  Toward an integration.  New

              

               York:  Basic Books.

 

                   Wachtel, P.L.  (1982).  Resistance:

                                        

               Psychodynamic and behavioral approaches.  New

              

               York:  Plenum Press.

 

                   Watson, J.B., & Rayner, R (1920).  Conditioned

 

                            emotional reactions.  Journal of Experimental

                                   

               Psychology, 3, 1-14.

              

                 

 

             Williams, J.M.G., Fraser, N.W., MacLeod, C.,

 

               Matthews A. (1988).  An information processing

                                   

               analysis of the emotional disorders.  New York:

              

               Wiley.

 

                   Winfrey, L.P.L., & Goldfried, M.R.  (1986).

 

               Information processing and the human change

 

               process.  In R.E.  Ingram (Ed.), Information

                                             

               processing approaches to clinical psychology (pp.

             

               241-260).  San Diego:  Academic Press.

 

                   Young E.J.  (1990).  Cognitive therapy for

                                      

               personality disorders:  A schema-focused approach.

              

               Sarasota:  Professional Resource Exchange.

 

                   Zajonc, R.B.  (1980).  Thinking and feeling:

 

               Preferences need no inferences.  American

                                            

               Psychologist, 35, 151-175.